Subscribe to RSS
DOI: 10.1055/s-0045-1806722
The Role of Rapid Automatized Naming in Children with Developmental Dysgraphia in German

Abstract
Various researchers have identified naming speed (rapid automatized naming [RAN]) as a predictor for reading skills and dyslexia. Although fewer studies investigate the connection between RAN and writing acquisition, their results suggest a connection between alphanumeric RAN tasks and spelling skills. Since the cognitive processes relevant to RAN have not yet been researched, it is unclear which components connect spelling performance and naming speed. Various authors propose a connection through orthographic and visual knowledge. This study investigated whether and how alphanumeric (letters and digits) and non-alphanumeric RAN (colors, objects) relate to spelling skills in German. Therefore, we investigated naming speed abilities in German 8- to 11-year-olds (n = 103) with pure developmental dysgraphia (i.e., isolated spelling deficit without reading deficit; n = 22), combined developmental dysgraphia and dyslexia (n = 26), and typical spelling and reading skills (n = 55). We found significant differences between children with pure developmental dysgraphia and children with typical reading and spelling skills for alphanumeric, but not non-alphanumeric RAN tasks. Our findings suggest that alphanumeric RAN, in contrast to non-alphanumeric RAN, is related to spelling. The study thus reveals the relevant difference between alphanumeric and non-alphanumeric RAN tasks and points toward distinct underlying cognitive mechanisms.
1 In this article, the terms developmental dysgraphia, dysgraphia, and spelling disorder are used synonymously and refer to difficulties in spelling acquisition in children.
2 The T distribution is a normal distribution with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. It is frequently used in standardized cognitive tests as a reference distribution to distinguish average performance from that above or below average. The T distribution can be transformed into the z distribution (mean: 0, standard deviation: 1), IQ distribution (mean: 100, standard deviation: 15), STANINE (mean: 5, standard deviation: 2), percentiles, etc. (see e.g., https://www.psychometrica.de/normwertrechner.html)
3 To reduce the risk false-positive results, the p-values for these multiple tests are reported with Bonferroni correction.
Publication History
Article published online:
21 May 2025
© 2025. Thieme. All rights reserved.
Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc.
333 Seventh Avenue, 18th Floor, New York, NY 10001, USA
-
References
-
Araújo, S., & Faísca, L. (2019). A meta-analytic review of naming-speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23(5), 349–368
-
Araújo, S., Reis, A., Petersson, K. M., & Faísca, L. (2015). Rapid automatized naming and reading performance: a meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 868–883
-
Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different?. Psychological Bulletin, 130(6), 858–886
-
Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 5(1), 69–85
-
Brudnicki, M. (2019). Früherkennung von Lese-Rechtschreibschwierigkeiten (LRS). Sprache, Stimme, Gehör, 43(4), 207–208
-
Bundesverband Legasthenie und Dyskalkulie e. V. (Ed.). ( (2018). Legasthenie: Ratgeber zum Thema Legasthenie-Erkennen und Verstehen
-
Chau, L. T., Dimitrova Vulchanova, M., & Talcott, J. B. (2021). To name or not to name: eye movements and semantic processing in RAN and reading. Brain Sciences, 11(7), 866
-
Chen, YJ. I., Thompson, C. G., Xu, Z., Irey, R. C., & Georgiou, G. K. (2021). Rapid automatized naming and spelling performance in alphabetic languages: a meta-analysis. Reading and Writing, 34(10), 2559–2580
-
Cummine, J., Szepesvari, E., Chouinard, B., Hanif, W., & Georgiou, G. K. (2014). A functional investigation of RAN letters, digits, and objects: How similar are they?. Behavioural Brain Research, 275, 157–165
-
Denckla, M. B., & Cutting, L. E. (1999). History and significance of rapid automatized naming. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 29–42
-
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10(2), 186–202
-
Deutsche Gesellschaft für Kinder und Jugendpsychiatrie, Psychosomatik und Psychotherapie e. V. (2015). S3 Leitlinie: Diagnostik und Behandlung von Kindern und Jugendlichen mit Lese- und/oder Rechtschreibstörung. Deutsche Gesellschaft für Kinder und Jugendpsychiatrie, Psychosomatik und Psychotherapie
-
Döhla, D., & Heim, S. (2016). Developmental dyslexia and dysgraphia: What can we learn from the one about the other?. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 2045
-
Döhla, D., Willmes, K., & Heim, S. (2018). Cognitive profiles of developmental dysgraphia. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2006
-
Donker, M., Kroesbergen, E., Slot, E., van Viersen, S., & de Bree, E. (2016). Alphanumeric and non-alphanumeric rapid automatized naming in children with reading and/or spelling difficulties and mathematical difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 80–87
-
Ehm, J-H., Kerner Auch Koerner, J., Gawrilow, C., Hasselhorn, M., & Schmiedek, F. (2016). The association of ADHD symptoms and reading acquisition during elementary school years. Developmental Psychology, 52(9), 1445–1456
-
Frith, U. (1986). A developmental framework for developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 36(1), 67–81
-
Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2010). Predicting reading and spelling difficulties in transparent and opaque orthographies: a comparison between Scandinavian and US/Australian children. Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 16(2), 119–142
-
Georgiou, G. K., & Parrila, R. (2020). What mechanism underlies the rapid automatized naming-reading relation?. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 194, 104840
-
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Cui, Y., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2013). Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading?. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 218–225
-
Goldston, D. B., Walsh, A., Mayfield Arnold, E., Reboussin, B., Sergent Daniel, S., Erkanli, A., Nutter, D., Hickman, E., Palmes, G., Snider, E., & Wood, F. B. (2007). Reading problems, psychiatric disorders, and functional impairment from mid- to late adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(1), 25–32
-
Gordon, P. C., & Hoedemaker, R. S. (2016). Effective scheduling of looking and talking during rapid automatized naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 42(5), 742–760
-
Grund, M., Haug, G., & Naumann, C. L. (2004). DRT 4: Diagnostischer Rechtschreibtest für 4. Klassen. Beltz
-
Heim, S., Tschierse, J., Amunts, K., Wilms, M., Vossel, S., Willmes, K., Grabowska, A., & Huber, W. (2008). Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 68(1), 73–82
-
Huschka, S. S., Georgiou, G. K., Brandenburg, J., Ehm, J-H., & Hasselhorn, M. (2021). Examining the contribution of RAN components to reading fluency, reading comprehension, and spelling in German. Reading and Writing, 34(9), 2317–2336
-
Khodadoust, M., Mohamadi, R., Janani, L., Javadi, Z., & Sadeghi, A. (2019). The effect of phonological awareness on rapid automatized naming. Medical Journal of the Islamic Republic of Iran, 33, 32
-
Landerl, K., Freudenthaler, H. H., Heene, M., de Jong, P. F., Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Parrila, R., & Georgiou, G. K. (2019). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming as longitudinal predictors of reading in five alphabetic orthographies with varying degrees of consistency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23(3), 220–234
-
Langer, N., Benjamin, C., Becker, B. L. C., & Gaab, N. (2019). Comorbidity of reading disabilities and ADHD: structural and functional brain characteristics. Human Brain Mapping, 40(9), 2677–2698
-
Martinez, D., Georgiou, G. K., Inoue, T., Falcón, A., & Parrila, R. (2021). How does rapid automatized naming influence orthographic knowledge?. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 204, 105064
-
Marx, H. (1998). Knuspels Leseaufgaben. Hogrefe
-
Mashal, N., & Gankin-Shwartz, R. (2024). Dyslexia: theories and brain-based interventions. In: Bennett G, Goodall E, eds. The Palgrave Encyclopedia of Disability. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham
-
Mayer, A., & Lindberg, S. (2021). Lese-Rechtschreibstörungen (LRS) (2. vollständig überarbeitete Auflage). UTB: Vol. 8662. Ernst Reinhardt Verlag
-
McCloskey, M., & Rapp, B. (2017). Developmental dysgraphia: an overview and framework for research. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34(3–4), 65–82
-
Mehlhase, H., Bakos, S., Landerl, K., Schulte-Körne, G., & Moll, K. (2019). Orthographic learning in children with isolated and combined reading and spelling deficits. Child Neuropsychology, 25(3), 370–393
-
Moll, K., Fussenegger, B., Willburger, E., & Landerl, K. (2009). RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing. evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(1), 1–25
-
Moll, K., Kunze, S., Neuhoff, N., Bruder, J., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2014). Specific learning disorder: prevalence and gender differences. PLoS One, 9(7), e103537
-
Mottier, G. (1951). Uber Untersuchungen der Sprache lesegestörter Kinder. Folia Phoniatrica, 3, 170–177.
-
Müller, R. (2003). Diagnostischer Rechtschreibtest für 3. Klassen: DRT 3. Beltz
-
Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2011). Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum. Cortex, 47(1), 117–127
-
Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences, 24(9), 508–511
-
Papadopoulos, T. C., Spanoudis, G. C., & Georgiou, G. K. (2016). How is RAN related to reading fluency? A comprehensive examination of the prominent theoretical accounts. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1217
-
Richards, T. L., Grabowski, T. J., Boord, P., Yagle, K., Askren, M., Mestre, Z., Robinson, P., Welker, O., Gulliford, D., Nagy, W., & Berninger, V. (2015). Contrasting brain patterns of writing-related DTI parameters, fMRI connectivity, and DTI-fMRI connectivity correlations in children with and without dysgraphia or dyslexia. Neuroimage Clinical, 8, 408–421
-
Rumsey, J. M., Horwitz, B., Donohue, B. C., Nace, K., Maisog, J. M., & Andreason, P. (1997). Phonological and orthographic components of word recognition. A PET-rCBF study. Brain, 120(Pt 5), 739–759
-
Savage, R., Pillay, V., & Melidona, S. (2008). Rapid serial naming is a unique predictor of spelling in children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(3), 235–250
-
Schaadt, G., Männel, C., van der Meer, E., Pannekamp, A., Oberecker, R., & Friederici, A. D. (2015). Present and past: Can writing abilities in school children be associated with their auditory discrimination capacities in infancy?. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 47, 318–333
-
Schulte-Körne, G. (2010). The prevention, diagnosis, and treatment of dyslexia. Deutsches Ärzteblatt International, 107(41), 718–726
-
Siegmüller, J., & von der Heide, B. (2015). Störungen des Lesens und Schreibens bei Kindern. In J. Siegmüller (Ed.), Leitfaden Sprache, Sprechen, Stimme, Schlucken (4., erw. Aufl., pp. 145–150). Elsevier Urban & Fischer
-
Snowling, M., & Hulme, C. (2006). Language skills, learning to read and reading intervention. London Review of Education, 4, 63–76
-
Song, S., Georgiou, G. K., Su, M., & Hua, S. (2015). How well do phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming correlate with Chinese reading accuracy and fluency? A meta-analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(2), 99–123
-
Stainthorp, R., Powell, D., & Stuart, M. (2013). The relationship between rapid naming and word spelling in English. Journal of Research in Reading, , doi: 10.1111/jrir.12002
-
Stein, J. (2001). The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 7(1), 12–36
-
Stein, J. (2014). Dyslexia: the role of vision and visual attention. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 1(4), 267–280
-
Steinbrink, C., Klatte, M., & Lachmann, T. (2014). Phonological, temporal and spectral processing in vowel length discrimination is impaired in German primary school children with developmental dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(11), 3034–3045
-
Stock, C., Marx, P., & Schneider, W. (2003). Basiskompetenzen für Lese-Rechtschreibleistungen: Ein Test zur Erfassung der phonologischen Bewusstheit vom ersten bis vierten Grundschuljahr. Hogrefe
-
van Ermingen-Marbach, M., Verhalen, N., Grande, M., Heim, S., Mayer, A., & Pape-Neumann, J. (2015). Orientierungswerte für die Benennungsgeschwindigkeit bei leseunauffälligen Kindern im Alter von 9 bis 11 Jahren. Sprache, 38(4), e28–e32
-
Vander Stappen, C., Dricot, L., & Van Reybroeck, M. (2020). RAN training in dyslexia: behavioral and brain correlates. Neuropsychologia, 146, 107566
-
Vander Stappen, C., & Reybroeck, M. V. (2018). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming are independent phonological competencies with specific impacts on word reading and spelling: an intervention study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 320
-
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1993). Development of young readers' phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(1), 83–103
-
Weiß, R. H. (2006). CFT 20-R mit WS/ZF-R: Grundintelligenztest Skala 2-Revision mit Wortschatztest und Zahlenfolgetest-Revision. Hogrefe
-
Winkes, J. (2014). Isolierte Rechtschreibstörung: Eigenständiges Störungsbild oder leichte Form der Rechtschreibstörung? Eine Untersuchung der kognitiv-linguistischen Informationsverarbeitungskompetenzen von Kindern mit Schriftspracherwerbsstörungen. (Vol. 2014). Universität Freiburg (Schweiz)
-
Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 415–438
-
Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: a conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 387–407
-
World Health Organisation (WHO) (2022). International Classification of Diseases Eleventh Revision (ICD-11). Geneva
-
Zarić, J., Hasselhorn, M., & Nagler, T. (2021). Orthographic knowledge predicts reading and spelling skills over and above general intelligence and phonological awareness. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 36(1), 21–43
-
Zoccolotti, P., & Friedmann, N. (2010). From dyslexia to dyslexias, from dysgraphia to dysgraphias, from a cause to causes: a look at current research on developmental dyslexia and dysgraphia. Cortex, 46(10), 1211–1215
-
Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Marinelli, C. V., & Spinelli, D. (2020). Predicting individual differences in reading, spelling and maths in a sample of typically developing children: a study in the perspective of comorbidity. PLoS One, 15(4), e0231937