Semin Speech Lang 2016; 37(04): 239-251
DOI: 10.1055/s-0036-1587708
Review Article
Thieme Medical Publishers 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA.

Pursuing Precision Speech-Language Therapy Services for Children with Down Syndrome

Jena McDaniel
1   Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee
,
Paul J. Yoder
2   Department of Special Education, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee
› Author Affiliations
Further Information

Publication History

Publication Date:
04 October 2016 (online)

Abstract

The behavioral phenotype of individuals with Down syndrome (DS) offers one avenue for developing speech-language therapy services that are tailored to the individual's characteristics that affect treatment response. Behavioral phenotypes are patterns of behavioral strengths and weaknesses for specific genetic disorders that can help guide the development and implementation of effective interventions. Nonetheless, individual differences within children with DS must be acknowledged and addressed because behavioral phenotypes are probabilistic, not deterministic. Developing precision speech-language therapy services to maximize learning opportunities and outcomes for children with DS calls for increased collaboration among clinicians and researchers to address the needs, challenges, and opportunities on three interconnected themes: (1) moving effective interventions from research to practice, (2) making evidence-based, child-specific treatment intensity decisions, and (3) considering child motivation and temperament characteristics. Increased availability of intervention materials and resources as well as more specific recommendations that acknowledge individual differences could help narrow the research-practice gap. Clear descriptions of disciplined manipulations of treatment intensity components could lead to more effective intervention services. Last, addressing motivation and temperament characteristics, such as the personality-motivation orientation, in children with DS may help maximize learning opportunities. Focused attention and collaboration on these key themes could produce substantial, positive changes for children with DS and their families in the coming decade.

 
  • References

  • 1 Obama B. State of the Union 2015: full transcript. CNN, January 2015. Available at: http://www.cnn.com/2015/01/20/politics/state-of-the-union-2015-transcript-full-text/ . Accessed May 5, 2016
  • 2 Fidler DJ, Nadel L. Education and children with Down syndrome: neuroscience, development, and intervention. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2007; 13 (3) 262-271
  • 3 Fidler D, Most D, Philofsky A. The Down syndrome behavioural phenotype: taking a developmental approach. Downs Syndr Res Pract 2009; 12: 37-44
  • 4 Butler J. The Analogy of Religion. New York, NY: Harper & Brothers; 1876
  • 5 Kumin L. Intelligibility of speech in children with Down syndrome in natural settings: parents' perspective. Percept Mot Skills 1994; 78 (1) 307-313
  • 6 Rosin MM, Swift E, Bless D, Vetter DK. Communication profiles of adolescents with Down syndrome. Commun Disord Quart 1988; 12: 49-64
  • 7 Yorkston KM, Strand EA, Kennedy MR. Comprehensibility of dysarthric speech: implications for assessment and treatment planning. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 1996; 5: 55-66
  • 8 Kent RD, Vorperian HK. Speech impairment in Down syndrome: a review. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2013; 56 (1) 178-210
  • 9 Kumin L. Speech intelligibility and childhood verbal apraxia in children with Down syndrome. Downs Syndr Res Pract 2006; 10 (1) 10-22
  • 10 Kumin L, Schoenbrodt L. Employment in adults with Down syndrome in the United States: results from a national survey. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil 2016; 29 (4) 330-345
  • 11 Miller JF. Individual differences in vocabulary acquisition in children with Down syndrome. Prog Clin Biol Res 1995; 393: 93-103
  • 12 Fowler AE. Language abilities in children with Down syndrome: evidence for a specific syntactic delay. In: Children with Down Syndrome: A Developmental Perspective. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1990: 302-328
  • 13 Hesketh LJ, Chapman RS. Verb use by individuals with Down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 1998; 103 (3) 288-304
  • 14 Chapman RS, Hesketh LJ. Language, cognition, and short-term memory in individuals with Down syndrome. Downs Syndr Res Pract 2001; 7 (1) 1-7
  • 15 Jarrold C, Baddeley AD, Phillips CE. Verbal short-term memory in Down syndrome: a problem of memory, audition, or speech?. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2002; 45 (3) 531-544
  • 16 Chapman RS, Schwartz SE, Bird EK-R. Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome: I. Comprehension. J Speech Hear Res 1991; 34 (5) 1106-1120
  • 17 Johnston F, Stansfield J. Expressive pragmatic skills in pre-school children with and without Down's syndrome: parental perceptions. J Intellect Disabil Res 1997; 41 (Pt 1) 19-29
  • 18 Abbeduto L, Murphy MM, Richmond EK , et al. Collaboration in referential communication: comparison of youth with Down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 2006; 111 (3) 170-183
  • 19 Carlesimo GA, Marotta L, Vicari S. Long-term memory in mental retardation: evidence for a specific impairment in subjects with Down's syndrome. Neuropsychologia 1997; 35 (1) 71-79
  • 20 Sigman M, Ruskin E, Arbeile S , et al. Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1999; 64 (1) 1-114
  • 21 Rosner BA, Hodapp RM, Fidler DJ, Sagun JN, Dykens EM. Social competence in persons with Prader-Willi, Williams and Down's syndromes. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil 2004; 17: 209-217
  • 22 Ruskin EM, Mundy P, Kasari C, Sigman M. Object mastery motivation of children with Down syndrome. Am J Ment Retard 1994; 98 (4) 499-509
  • 23 Rothbart MK, Derryberry D. Development of individual differences in temperament. Advances in developmental psychology. In: Lamb ME, Brown A, , eds. Advances in Developmental Psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1981: 37-86
  • 24 Gartstein MA, Marmion J, Swanson HL. Infant temperament: an evaluation of children with Down syndrome. J Reprod Infant Psychol 2006; 24: 31-41
  • 25 Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev 2003; 26: 64-86
  • 26 Gunn P, Cuskelly M. Down syndrome temperament: the stereotype at middle childhood and adolescence. Int J Disabil Dev Educ 1991; 38: 59-70
  • 27 Bridges FA, Cicchetti D. Mothers' ratings of the temperament characteristics of Down syndrome infants. Dev Psychol 1982; 18: 238-244
  • 28 Hepburn S, Philofsky A, Fidler DJ, Rogers S. Autism symptoms in toddlers with Down syndrome: a descriptive study. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil 2008; 21 (1) 48-57
  • 29 Olswang LB, Prelock PA. Bridging the gap between research and practice: implementation science. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2015; 58 (6) S1818-S1826
  • 30 Grol R, Wensing M, Eccles M, Davis D. Improving Patient Care: The Implementation of Change in Health Care. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2013
  • 31 Green LW, Ottoson JM, García C, Hiatt RA. Diffusion theory and knowledge dissemination, utilization, and integration in public health. Annu Rev Public Health 2009; 30: 151-174
  • 32 Fey ME, Yoder PJ, Warren SF, Bredin-Oja SL. Is more better? Milieu communication teaching in toddlers with intellectual disabilities. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2013; 56 (2) 679-693
  • 33 Warren SF, Bredin-Oja SL, Fairchild M , et al. Responsivity education/prelinguistic milieu teaching. In: McCauley RJ, Fey ME, , eds. Treatment of Language Disorders in Children. Baltimore, MD: Brooks; 2006: 47-75
  • 34 Warren SF, Fey ME, Finestack LH, Brady NC, Bredin-Oja SL, Fleming KK. A randomized trial of longitudinal effects of low-intensity responsivity education/prelinguistic milieu teaching. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2008; 51 (2) 451-470
  • 35 Fey ME, Warren SF, Brady N , et al. Early effects of responsivity education/prelinguistic milieu teaching for children with developmental delays and their parents. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2006; 49 (3) 526-547
  • 36 Hancock T, Kaiser A. Treatment of language disorders in children. In: McCauley RJ, Fey ME, , eds. Treatment of Language Disorders in Children. Baltimore, MD: Brooks; 2006: 203-236
  • 37 Yoder PJ, Camarata S, Woynaroski T. Treating speech comprehensibility in students with Down syndrome. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2016; 59 (3) 446-459
  • 38 Camarata S, Yoder P, Camarata M. Simultaneous treatment of grammatical and speech-comprehensibility deficits in children with Down syndrome. Downs Syndr Res Pract 2006; 11 (1) 9-17
  • 39 Yoder P, Camarata S, Gardner E. Treatment effects on speech intelligibility and length of utterance in children with specific language and intelligibility impairments. J Early Interv 2005; 28: 34-49
  • 40 Pelatti CY. Enhancing oral and written language for adolescents and young adults with Down syndrome. Semin Speech Lang 2015; 36 (1) 50-59
  • 41 Chambers DA, Glasgow RE, Stange KC. The dynamic sustainability framework: addressing the paradox of sustainment amid ongoing change. Implement Sci 2013; 8: 117
  • 42 Warren SF, Fey ME, Yoder PJ. Differential treatment intensity research: a missing link to creating optimally effective communication interventions. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2007; 13 (1) 70-77
  • 43 Baker E. Optimal intervention intensity in speech-language pathology: discoveries, challenges, and unchartered territories. Int J Speech-Language Pathol 2012; 14 (5) 478-485
  • 44 Baker E. Optimal intervention intensity. Int J Speech-Language Pathol 2012; 14 (5) 401-409
  • 45 Brandel J, Frome Loeb D. Program intensity and service delivery models in the schools: SLP survey results. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2011; 42 (4) 461-490
  • 46 Yoder P, Fey ME, Warren SF. Studying the impact of intensity is important but complicated. Int J Speech-Language Pathol 2012; 14 (5) 410-413
  • 47 Kamhi AG. Improving clinical practices for children with language and learning disorders. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2014; 45 (2) 92-103
  • 48 Childers JB, Tomasello M. Two-year-olds learn novel nouns, verbs, and conventional actions from massed or distributed exposures. Dev Psychol 2002; 38 (6) 967-978
  • 49 Riches NG, Tomasello M, Conti-Ramsden G. Verb learning in children with SLI: frequency and spacing effects. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2005; 48 (6) 1397-1411
  • 50 Ambridge B, Theakston AL, Lieven EV , et al. The distributed learning effect for children's acquisition of an abstract syntactic construction. Cogn Dev 2006; 21: 174-193
  • 51 Daunhauer LA, Fidler DJ. The Down syndrome behavioral phenotype: implications for practice and research in occupational therapy. Occup Ther Health Care 2011; 25 (1) 7-25
  • 52 Wishart J. Learning the hard way: avoidance strategies in young children with Down syndrome. Downs Syndr Res Pract 1993; 1: 47-55
  • 53 Wishart J. Avoidant learning styles and cognitive development in young children. In: Stratford B, Gunn P, , eds. New Approaches to Down Syndrome. London, UK: Cassell; 1996: 173-205
  • 54 Wishart J. Motivation and learning styles in young children with Down syndrome. Downs Syndr Res Pract 2001; 7 (2) 47-51
  • 55 Wishart JG, Johnston FH. The effects of experience on attribution of a stereotyped personality to children with Down's syndrome. J Ment Defic Res 1990; 34 (Pt 5): 409-420
  • 56 Drake M. Easy Does It for Articulation: A Phonological Approach. East Moine, IL: LinguiSystems; 2002
  • 57 Yoder PJ, Layton TL. Speech following sign language training in autistic children with minimal verbal language. J Autism Dev Disord 1988; 18 (2) 217-229
  • 58 Warren SF, Gilkerson J, Richards JA , et al. What automated vocal analysis reveals about the vocal production and language learning environment of young children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2010; 40 (5) 555-569
  • 59 Oller DK, Niyogi P, Gray S , et al. Automated vocal analysis of naturalistic recordings from children with autism, language delay, and typical development. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2010; 107 (30) 13354-13359
  • 60 Thiemann-Bourque KS, Warren SF, Brady N, Gilkerson J, Richards JA. Vocal interaction between children with Down syndrome and their parents. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2014; 23 (3) 474-485
  • 61 Dykstra JR, Sabatos-Devito MG, Irvin DW, Boyd BA, Hume KA, Odom SL. Using the Language Environment Analysis (LENA) system in preschool classrooms with children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism 2013; 17 (5) 582-594
  • 62 Aragon M, Yoshinaga-Itano C. Using Language ENvironment Analysis to improve outcomes for children who are deaf or hard of hearing. Semin Speech Lang 2012; 33 (4) 340-353
  • 63 Suskind DL, Leffel KR, Graf E , et al. A parent-directed language intervention for children of low socioeconomic status: a randomized controlled pilot study. J Child Lang 2015; 4: 1-41
  • 64 Woynaroski T. The Stability and Validity of Automated Vocal Analysis in Preschoolers with Autism Spectrum Disorder in the Early Stages of Language Development [dissertation]. Nashville, TN, Vanderbilt University; 2014
  • 65 Yoder P, Woynaroski T, Fey M, Warren S. Effects of dose frequency of early communication intervention in young children with and without Down syndrome. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil 2014; 119 (1) 17-32
  • 66 Godsil RD, Tropp LR, Goff PA, Powell JA. The Science of Equality, Vol. 1: Addressing Implicit Bias, Racial Anxiety, and Stereotype Threat in Education and Healthcare. Perception Institute; 2014 . Available at: http://perception.org/app/uploads/2014/11/Science-of-Equality-111214_web.pdf . Accessed August 11, 2016
  • 67 Libertus K, Joh AS, Needham AW. Motor training at 3 months affects object exploration 12 months later. Dev Sci 2015; xx: 1-9
  • 68 Needham A, Joh AS, Wiesen SE, Williams N. Effects of contingent reinforcement of actions on infants' object-directed reaching. Infancy 2014; 19: 496-517