Nervenheilkunde 2005; 24(09): 829-836
DOI: 10.1055/s-0038-1630013
Original Article
Schattauer GmbH

EEG-Charakteristika von motorischen Imaginationen in Hypnose

EEG characteristics of motor imagery during hypnosis
O. B. Scholz
1   Lehrstuhl für Klinische und Angewandte Psychologie der Rheinischen Friedrich-Wilhelms-Universität Bonn (Direktor: Prof. Dr. O. B. Scholz)
,
B. Konradt
1   Lehrstuhl für Klinische und Angewandte Psychologie der Rheinischen Friedrich-Wilhelms-Universität Bonn (Direktor: Prof. Dr. O. B. Scholz)
› Author Affiliations
Further Information

Publication History

Eingegangen am: 01 August 2004

angenommen am: 18 November 2004

Publication Date:
24 January 2018 (online)

Zusammenfassung

Im Unterschied zur praktischen Evidenz und klaren neurophysiologischen Vorstellungen über die Beziehungen zwischen motorischer Imaginationsfähigkeit und Hypnotisierbarkeit steht der empirische Nachweis dieses Zusammenhangs noch aus. Methodische Unzulänglichkeiten scheinen für diese Sachlage verantwortlich zu sein. Mittels mentaler Chronometrie kann eine Standardisierung und Objektivierung motorischer Imagination während des hypnotischen Prozedere gewährleistet werden. Die Ableitung des EEG liefert objektive Indikatoren für die hypnotische Trance. Alpha- und Thetaaktivität des Spontan-EEG sind bisher maßgeblich untersucht worden. Unter fünf Fragestellungen werden diesbezügliche Literaturbefunde dargestellt. Anhand einer eigenen Studie wird ein neues Vorgehen demonstriert. Die Ergebnisse sind sowohl methodisch als auch inhaltlich vielversprechend. Die anwendungsbezogenen Schlussfolgerungen werden diskutiert.

Summary

Although practical experiences and neurophysiological considerations support a strong link between imagery and hypnosis, no empirical evidence for this relationship could be found so far. Methodical deficiencies seem to be responsible for this lack of evidence. Mental chronometry can be used to standardise and objectify measurement of motor imagery during the hypnotic process. The recording of continuous EEG provides objective markers of hypnotic trance as well as imaginative processing. Alpha- and theta activity have been mostly researched in this context. Within the framework of five questions findings from literature will be reviewed. An empirical study will demonstrate an improved research strategy. Results are promising methodically as well as regards content. Conclusions for clinical practice are discussed.

 
  • Literatur

  • 1 Akinpar S, Ulett GA, Itil TM. Hypnotizability predicted by digital computer-analyzed EEG pattern. Biological Psychiatry 1971; 3: 387-92.
  • 2 Barnier AJ, McConkey KM. Hypnosis, human nature, and complexity: Integrating neuroscience approaches into hypnosis research. The International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 2003; 51: 282-308.
  • 3 Baar E, Schürmann M, Sakowitz O. The selectively distributed theta system: functions. International Journal of Psychophysiology 2001; 39: 197-212.
  • 4 Beyer L. et al. Dynamics of central nervous activation during motor imagination. International Journal of Psychophysiology 1990; 9: 75-80.
  • 5 Comey G, Kirsch I. Intentional and spontaneous imagery in hypnosis: The phenomenology of hypnotic responding. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 1999; 47: 65-85.
  • 6 Crawford HJ, Gruzelier J. A midstream view of the psychoneurophysiology of hypnosis: Recent research and future directions. In Fromm E, Nash M. (Eds) Hypnosis: Research developments and perspectives. New York: Guilford; 1992: 227-66.
  • 7 Crawford HJ. Cognitive and physiological flexibility: Multiple pathways to hypnotic responsiveness. In Gheorghiu V, Netter P, Eysenck H, Rosenthal R. (Eds) Suggestion and suggestibility: Theory and research. New York: Springer Verlag; 1989: 155-68.
  • 8 Crawford HJ, Clarke SW, Kitner-Triolo M. Selfgenerated happy and sad emotions in low and highly hypnotizable persons during waking and hypnosis: laterality and regional EEG activity differences. International Journal of Psychophysiology 1996; 24: 239-66.
  • 9 Davidson R, Schwartz GE. The influence of musical training on patterns of EEG asymmetry during musical and non-musical self-generation task. Psychophysiology 1977; 14: 58-63.
  • 10 DePascalis V, Ray WJ, Tranquillo I, D’Amico D. EEG Activity and heart rate during recall of emotional events in hypnosis: relationships with hypnotizability and suggestibility. International Journal of Psychophysiology 1998; 29: 255-75.
  • 11 Decety J. The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behavioral Brain Research 1996; 77: 45-52.
  • 12 Decety J, Jeannerod M. Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitts’ law hold in motor imagery?. Behavioural Brain Research 1996; 72: 127-34.
  • 13 Decety J, Ingvar DH. Brain stuctures participating in mental simulation of motor behavior: a neuropsychological interpretation. Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam) 1990; 73: 13-34.
  • 14 DePascalis V, Perrone M. EEG asymmetry and heart rate during experience of hypnotic analgesia in high an low hypnotizables. International Journal of Psychophysiology 1996; 21: 163-75.
  • 15 DePascalis V, Imperiali MC. Personality, hypnotic susceptibility and EEG responses. preliminary study. Perceptual and Motor Skills 1984; 59: 371-78.
  • 16 DePascalis V, Palumbo G. EEG alpha asymmetry: Task difficulty and hypnotizability. Perceptual and Motor Skills 1986; 62: 139-50.
  • 17 Evans FJ. Hypnosis and sleep: techniques for exploring cognitive activity during sleep. In Fromm E, Shor RE. (Eds) Hypnosis: Developments in Research and New Perspectives. 2nd edn. Aldine; New York: 1979: 139-83.
  • 18 Farthing GW, Venturino M, Brown SW. Relationship between two different types of imagery vividness questionnaire items and three hypnotic susceptibility scale factors: a brief communication. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 1983; 31: 8-13.
  • 19 Fitts PM. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology 1954; 47: 381.
  • 20 Germain A, Nielsen T. EEG Power Associated with Early Sleep Onset Images Differing in Sensory Content. Sleep Research Online 2001; 4: 83-90.
  • 21 Glisky ML, Tataryn DJ, Kihlstrom JF. Hypnotizability and mental imagery. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 1995; 43: 34-54.
  • 22 Graffin NF, Ray WJ, Lundy R. EEG Concomitants of Hypnosis and Hypnotic Susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1995; 104: 123-31.
  • 23 Isaac AR, Marks DF, Russell DG. An instrument of assessing imagery of movement: the vividness of movement questionnaire. Journal of Mental Imagery 1986; 10: 23-30.
  • 24 Jasper HH. Report of the committee on methods of clinical examination in elecroencephalography. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1958; 10: 370-5.
  • 25 Konradt B, Deeb S, Scholz OB. Motorische Imagination in Hypnose: Alpha- und Thetapower während motorischer Imagination in Hypnose. Hypnose und Kognition. (im Druck)
  • 26 Lynch JJ, Paskewitz DA, Orne MT. Some factors in the feedback control of human alpha rhythm. Psychosomatic Medicine 1974; 36: 399-410.
  • 27 MacLeod-Morgan C, Lack L. Hemispheric specificity: A physiological concomitant of hypnotizability. Psychophysiology 1982; 19: 687-90.
  • 28 Marks DF, Isaac AR. Topographical distribution of EEG activity accompanying visual and motor imagery in vivid and non-vivid imagers. British Journal of Psychology 1995; 86: 271-82.
  • 29 Nowlis DP, Rhead JC. Relation of eyes-closed resting EEG alpha activity to hypnotic susceptibility. Perceptual and Motor Skills 1968; 27: 1047-50.
  • 30 Page RA. Mental Imagery, Hypnotizability, Differential Item Difficulty, and Context Effects. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 1998; 41: 162-67.
  • 31 Papaxanthis C, Schieppati M, Gentili R, Pozzo T. Imagined and actual arm movements have similar durations when performed under different conditions of directions and mass. Experimental Brain Research; ( link.springer.de/link/service/journals/00221/contents/s00221–002–1012–1ch100.htm 16.02.2002).
  • 32 Paskewitz DA. EEG alpha activity and its relationship to altered states of consciousness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1977; 296: 151-64.
  • 33 Perlini AH, Spanos NP. EEG Alpha methodologies and hypnotizability: A critical review. Psychophysiology 1991; 28: 511-30.
  • 34 Sabourin ME, Cutcomb S, Crawford HJ, Pribram K. EEG correlates of hypnotic susceptibility and hypnotic trance: spectral analysis and coherence. International Journal of Psychophysiology 1990; 10: 125-42.
  • 35 Schürmann M, Basar E. Functional aspects of alpha oscillations in the EEG. International Journal of Psychophysiology 2001; 39: 151-58.
  • 36 Tebecis AK, Provins KA, Fanbach RW, Pentony P. Hypnosis and the EEG. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 1975; 161: 1-17.
  • 37 Ulett GA, Akpinar S, Itil TM. Quantitative EEG analysis during hypnosis. Electorencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology 1972; b 33: 361-368.
  • 38 Weiss T, Beyer L, Hansen E, Rost R, Paproth A. Motor Imagination: Dynamic of some neurophysiological correlates. Neuroscience 1987; 22: 511.
  • 39 Williams JD, Gruzelier JH. Differentiation of hypnosis and relaxation by analysis of narrow band theta and alpha frequencies. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 2001; 49: 185-206.
  • 40 Williams JD, Rippon G, Stone BM, Annett J. Psychophysiological correlates of dynamic imagery. British Journal of Psychology 1995; 86: 283-300.