Int J Sports Med 2016; 37(05): 341-346
DOI: 10.1055/s-0035-1565139
Physiology & Biochemistry
© Georg Thieme Verlag KG Stuttgart · New York

Using Deception to Establish a Reproducible Improvement in 4-Km Cycling Time Trial Performance

R.-J. Shei
1   Department of Kinesiology, Indiana University, Bloomington, United States
,
K. Thompson
2   University of Canberra, UC National Institute of Sport Studies, Canberra, Australia
,
R. Chapman
1   Department of Kinesiology, Indiana University, Bloomington, United States
,
J. Raglin
1   Department of Kinesiology, Indiana University, Bloomington, United States
,
T. Mickleborough
1   Department of Kinesiology, Indiana University, Bloomington, United States
› Author Affiliations
Further Information

Publication History



accepted after revision 07 October 2015

Publication Date:
08 February 2016 (online)

Abstract

We investigated whether performance gains achieved with deception persisted after the deception was revealed, and whether pacing strategy changed. 14 trained cyclists completed 4 simulated 4-km time trials (TT) on a cycle ergometer comprising familiarization and baseline trials (BAS), followed by “unaware” (of deception, UAW) and “aware” (of deception, AW) trials on separate days. In the UAW trial, participants competed against an on-screen avatar set at 102% of their baseline trial mean power output (Pmean) believing it was set at 100% of BAS Pmean. 24 h prior to the AW trial, participants were informed of the deception in the UAW trial. 4 participants did not improve in the UAW trial compared to BAS. 10 participants improved time to completion (TTC) and Pmean in the UAW and AW trials compared to BAS (p<0.03) with no significant differences between UAW and AW (p=1.0). Pacing strategy (at 0.5-km intervals) and RPE responses were unchanged (p>0.05) for these participants. In summary, deception did not improve performance in all participants. However, participants whose time trial performance improved following deception could retain their performance gains once the deception was revealed, demonstrating a similar pacing strategy and RPE response.

 
  • References

  • 1 Amann M, Proctor LT, Sebranek JJ, Pegelow DF, Dempsey JA. Opioid-mediated muscle afferents inhibit central motor drive and limit peripheral muscle fatigue development in humans. J Physiol 2009; 587: 271-283
  • 2 Borg GAV. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1982; 14: 377-381
  • 3 Cafarelli E. Peripheral contributions to the perception of effort. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1982; 14: 382-389
  • 4 Coquart JB, Garcin M. Knowledge of the endpoint: effect on perceptual values. Int J Sports Med 2008; 29: 976-979
  • 5 Corbett J, Barwood MJ, Ouzounoglou A, Thelwell R, Dicks M. Influence of competition on performance and pacing during cycling exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2012; 44: 509-515
  • 6 de Koning JJ, Foster C, Bakkum A, Kloppenburg S, Thiel C, Joseph T, Cohen J, Porcari JP. Regulation of pacing strategy during athletic competition. PLoS ONE 2011; 6: e15863
  • 7 Dishman R, Ickes W. Self-motivation and adherence to therapeutic exercise. J Behav Med 1981; 4: 421-438
  • 8 Dishman RK, Ickes W, Morgan WP. Self-Motivation and adherence to habitual physical activity1. J Appl Soc Psych 1980; 10: 115-132
  • 9 Duke J, Stickford J, Weavil J, Chapman R, Stager J, Mickleborough T. Operating lung volumes are affected by exercise mode but not trunk and hip angle during maximal exercise. Eur J Appl Physiol 2014; 114: 2387-2397
  • 10 Foster C, Schrager M, Snyder AC, Thompson NN. Pacing strategy and athletic performance. Sports Med 1994; 17: 77-85
  • 11 Gandevia SC. Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol Rev 2001; 81: 1725-1789
  • 12 Harriss DJ, Atkinson G. Ethical standards in sport and exercise science research. 2016 update Int J Sports Med 2015; 36: 1121-1124
  • 13 Heiby EM, Onorato VA, Sato RA. Cross-validation of the Self-Motivation Inventory. J Sport Psych 1987; 9: 394-399
  • 14 Howley ET, Bassett Jr DR, Welch HG. Criteria for maximal oxygen uptake: review and commentary. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1995; 27: 1292-1301
  • 15 Jones H, Williams E, Bridge C, Marchant D, Midgley A, Micklewright D, Mc Naughton L. Physiological and psychological effects of deception on pacing strategy and performance: A review. Sports Med 2013; 43: 1243-1257
  • 16 Mauger AR, Jones AM, Williams CA. Influence of acetaminophen on performance during time trial cycling. J Appl Physiol 2010; 108: 98-104
  • 17 Morton RH. Deception by manipulating the clock calibration influences cycle ergometer endurance time in males. J Sci Med Sport 2009; 12: 332-337
  • 18 Ness RG, Patton RW. The effect of beliefs on maximum weight-lifting performance. Cogn Ther Res 1979; 3: 205-211
  • 19 Noakes TD. Time to move beyond a brainless exercise physiology: the evidence for a complex regulation of human exercise performance. Appl Physiol Nutr Met 2011; 36: 23-35
  • 20 Noakes TD. Fatigue is a brain-derived emotion that regulates the exercise behavior to ensure the protection of whole body homeostasis. Front Physiol 2012; 3: 1-13
  • 21 Noakes TD, St Clair Gibson A. Logical limitations to the “catastrophe” models of fatigue during exercise in humans. Br J Sport Med 2004; 38: 648-649
  • 22 Noakes TD, St Clair Gibson A, Lambert EV. From catastrophe to complexity: a novel model of integrative central neural regulation of effort and fatigue during exercise in humans: summary and conclusions. Br J Sport Med 2005; 39: 120-124
  • 23 Parry D, Chinnasamy C, Micklewright D. Optic flow influences perceived exertion during cycling. J Sport Exerc Psychol 2012; 34: 444-456
  • 24 Raglin JS, Morgan WP, Luchsinger AE. Mood and self-motivation in successful and unsuccessful female rowers. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1990; 22: 849-853
  • 25 St Clair Gibson A, De Koning JJ, Thompson KG, Roberts WO, Micklewright D, Raglin J, Foster C. Crawling to the finish line: why do endurance runners collapse?: implications for understanding of mechanisms underlying pacing and fatigue. Sports Med 2013; 43: 413-424
  • 26 St Clair Gibson A, Noakes TD. Evidence for complex system integration and dynamic neural regulation of skeletal muscle recruitment during exercise in humans. Br J Sport Med 2004; 38: 797-806
  • 27 Stone MR, Thomas K, Wilkinson M, Jones AM, St Clair Gibson A, Thompson KG. Effects of deception on exercise performance: implications for determinants of fatigue in humans. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2012; 44: 534-541
  • 28 Stone MR, Thomas K, Wilkinson M, St Clair Gibson A, Thompson KG. Consistency of perceptual and metabolic responses to a laboratory-base simulated 4,000-m cycling time trial. Eur J Appl Physiol 2011; 111: 1807-1813
  • 29 Swart J, Lamberts RP, Lambert MI, Lambert EV, Woolrich RW, Johnston S, Noakes TD. Exercising with reserve: exercise regulation by perceived exertion in relation to duration of exercise and knowledge of endpoint. Br J Sport Med 2009; 43: 775-781
  • 30 Thomas G, Renfree A. The effect of secret clock manipulation on 10km cycle time trial performance. Int J Arts Sci 2010; 3: 193-202
  • 31 Tucker R. The anticipatory regulation of performance: the physiological basis for pacing strategies and the development of a perception-based model for exercise performance. Br J Sport Med 2009; 43: 392-400
  • 32 Tucker R, Noakes TD. The physiological regulation of pacing strategy during exercise: a critical review. Br J Sport Med 2009; 43: e1
  • 33 Ulmer H-V. Concept of an extracellular regulation of muscular metabolic rate during heavy exercise in humans by psychophysiological feedback. Cell Molec Life Sci 1996; 52: 416-420
  • 34 Williams EL, Jones HS, Sparks SA, Marchant D, Micklewright D, Mc Naughton LR. Deception studies manipulating centrally acting performance modifiers: A review. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2014; 46: 1441-1451