Semin Speech Lang 2014; 35(2): 114-131
DOI: 10.1055/s-0034-1371755
Thieme Medical Publishers 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA.

Temperament, Emotion, and Childhood Stuttering

Robin Jones
1   Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences
,
Dahye Choi
1   Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences
,
Edward Conture
1   Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences
,
Tedra Walden
2   Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee
› Author Affiliations
Further Information

Publication History

Publication Date:
29 April 2014 (online)

Abstract

Reactivity refers to arousal of emotions, motor activity, and attention, and self-regulation refers to the ability to moderate those tendencies. In general, temperament is typically thought of as an individual's constitutionally (biologically) based behavioral proclivities. These proclivities often include emotional reactivity and self-regulation. Reactivity refers to arousal of emotions, motor activity, and attention, and self-regulation refers to the ability to moderate those tendencies. The traitlike nature of temperament makes it potentially salient to our understanding of the onset and development of stuttering because temperamental tendencies may result in greater reactivity or difficulty in coping. Emotions, which are more statelike and variable, may influence the variation of stuttering commonly observed both within and between speaking situations. Temperament and emotion may serve as a causal contributor to developmental stuttering, with empirical findings indicating that preschool-aged children who stutter (CWS) exhibit differences in temperament and emotion when compared with children who do not stutter. Given that empirical study of temperament in preschool-aged CWS is nascent, extensive discussion of clinical implications is challenging. With that caution, we present some early possibilities, including matching treatment approaches with the child's temperamental profile and using temperament as a predictor of treatment outcome.

 
  • References

  • 1 Conture EG, Kelly EM, Walden TA. Temperament, speech and language: an overview. J Commun Disord 2013; 46 (2) 125-142
  • 2 Conture EG, Walden TA. Dual diathesis-stressor model of stuttering. In: Beliakova L, Filatova Y, , eds. Theoretical Issues of Fluency Disorders. Moscow, Russia: Vlados; 2012: 94-127
  • 3 Goldsmith HH, Buss AH, Plomin R , et al. Roundtable: what is temperament? Four approaches. Child Dev 1987; 58 (2) 505-529
  • 4 Rothbart MK, Derryberry D. Development of individual differences in temperament. In: Lamb ME, Brown AL, , eds. Advances in Developmental Psychology. Vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1981: 37-86
  • 5 Rothbart MK. Temperament, development, and personality. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2007; 16 (4) 207-212
  • 6 Sanson A, Hemphill SA, Smart D. Connections between temperament and social development: a review. Soc Dev 2004; 13 (1) 142-170
  • 7 Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Eisenberg N, , ed. Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development. Vol. 3, 5th ed. New York, NY: Wiley; 1998: 105-176
  • 8 Saudino KJ. Behavioral genetics and child temperament. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2005; 26 (3) 214-223
  • 9 Cole PM, Martin SE, Dennis TA. Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Dev 2004; 75 (2) 317-333
  • 10 Campos JJ, Frankel CB, Camras L. On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Dev 2004; 75 (2) 377-394
  • 11 Spinrad TL, Eisenberg N, Harris E , et al. The relation of children's everyday nonsocial peer play behavior to their emotionality, regulation, and social functioning. Dev Psychol 2004; 40 (1) 67-80
  • 12 Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1994; 59 (2–3) 25-52
  • 13 LeDoux J. The Emotional Brain. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster; 1996
  • 14 Kagan J. Biology and the child. In: Eisenberg N, , ed. Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development. Vol. 3, 5th ed. New York, NY: Wiley; 1998: 177-235
  • 15 Gunnar MR. Psychoendocrine studies of temperament and stress in early childhood: expanding current models. In: Bates JE, Wachs TD, , eds. Temperament: Individual Differences at the Interface of Biology and Behavior. APA Science Volumes. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1994: 175-198
  • 16 Fowles DC, Kochanska G, Murray K. Electrodermal activity and temperament in preschool children. Psychophysiology 2000; 37 (6) 777-787
  • 17 Porges SW, Doussard-Roosevelt JA, Portales AL, Suess PE. Cardiac vagal tone: stability and relation to difficultness in infants and 3-year-olds. Dev Psychobiol 1994; 27 (5) 289-300
  • 18 Blackford JU, Allen AH, Cowan RL, Avery SN. Amygdala and hippocampus fail to habituate to faces in individuals with an inhibited temperament. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 8 (2) 143-150
  • 19 Marshall P, Fox N. Infant EEG and ERP in relation to social and emotional development. In: de Haan M, , ed. Infant EEG and Event-Related Potentials. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2007: 227-249
  • 20 Campbell DT, Fiske DW. Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychol Bull 1959; 56 (2) 81-105
  • 21 McDevitt SC, Carey WB. The measurement of temperament in 3–7.  year old children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1978; 19 (3) 245-253
  • 22 Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P. Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Dev 2001; 72 (5) 1394-1408
  • 23 Buss AH, Plomin R. Temperament: Early Developing Personality Traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1984
  • 24 Windle M, Lerner RM. Reassessing the dimensions of temperamental individuality across the life span: the Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS-R). J Adolesc Res 1986; 1 (2) 213-229
  • 25 Strelau J. Temperament, Personality and Activity. London, England, New York, NY: Academic Press; 1983
  • 26 Henderson HA, Wachs TD. Temperament theory and the study of cognition–emotion interactions across development. Dev Rev 2007; 27 (3) 396-427
  • 27 Goldsmith H, Rothbart MK. Prelocomotor and Locomotor Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB; version 3.0; Technical Manual). Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin, Department of Psychology; 1996
  • 28 Tapp J, Walden T. PROCODER: a professional tape control, coding, and analysis system for behavioral research using videotape. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 1993; 25 (1) 53-56
  • 29 Zimmerman PH, Bolhuis JE, Willemsen A, Meyer ES, Noldus LP. The Observer XT: a tool for the integration and synchronization of multimodal signals. Behav Res Methods 2009; 41 (3) 731-735
  • 30 Rothbart MK, Goldsmith HH. Three approaches to the study of infant temperament. Dev Rev 1985; 5 (3) 237-260
  • 31 El-Sheikh M, Kouros CD, Erath S, Cummings EM, Keller P, Staton L. Marital conflict and children's externalizing behavior: interactions between parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous system activity. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 2009; 74: 1-79
  • 32 Doussard-Roosevelt JA, Montgomery LA, Porges SW. Short-term stability of physiological measures in kindergarten children: respiratory sinus arrhythmia, heart period, and cortisol. Dev Psychobiol 2003; 43 (3) 230-242
  • 33 Porges SW. The polyvagal perspective. Biol Psychol 2007; 74 (2) 116-143
  • 34 Treon M. Revised stuttering and related TPNB-LPPD threshold syndrome hypothesis in light of recent research. Psychology and Education Journal 2010; 47 (3–4) 1-27
  • 35 Rieser-Danner LA. Individual differences in infant fearfulness and cognitive performance: a testing, performance, or competence effect?. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr 2003; 129 (1) 41-71
  • 36 Salley BJ, Dixon Jr WE. Temperamental and joint attentional predictors of language development. Merrill Palmer Q (Wayne State Univ Press) 2007; 53 (1) 131-154
  • 37 Bloom L, Capatides JB. Expression of affect and the emergence of language. Child Dev 1987; 58 (6) 1513-1522
  • 38 Dixon WEJ, Shore C. Temperamental predictors of linguistic style during multiword acquisition. Infant Behav Dev 1997; 20 (1) 99-103
  • 39 Dixon WEJ, Smith PH. Links between early temperament and language acquisition. Merrill-Palmer Q 2000; 46 (3) 417-440
  • 40 Morales M, Mundy P, Delgado CE, Yale M, Neal R, Schwartz HK. Gaze following, temperament, and language development in 6-month-olds: a replication and extension. Infant Behav Dev 2000; 23 (2) 231-236
  • 41 Morales M, Mundy P, Delgado CEF , et al. Responding to joint attention across the 6- through 24-month age period and early language acquisition. J Appl Dev Psychol 2000; 21 (3) 283-298
  • 42 Noel M, Peterson C, Jesso B. The relationship of parenting stress and child temperament to language development among economically disadvantaged preschoolers. J Child Lang 2008; 35 (4) 823-843
  • 43 Usai MC, Garello V, Viterbori P. Temperamental profiles and linguistic development: differences in the quality of linguistic production in relation to temperament in children of 28 months. Infant Behav Dev 2009; 32 (3) 322-330
  • 44 Slomkowski CL, Nelson K, Dunn J, Plomin R. Temperament and language: Relations from toddlerhood to middle childhood. Dev Psychol 1992; 28 (6) 1090-1095
  • 45 Kagan J, Reznick JS, Gibbons J. Inhibited and uninhibited types of children. Child Dev 1989; 60 (4) 838-845
  • 46 Kagan J, Reznick JS, Clarke C, Snidman N, Garcia-Coll C. Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Dev 1984; 55 (6) 2212-2225
  • 47 Kagan J, Snidman N, Arcus D. Childhood derivatives of high and low reactivity in infancy. Child Dev 1998; 69 (6) 1483-1493
  • 48 Harrison LJ, McLeod S. Risk and protective factors associated with speech and language impairment in a nationally representative sample of 4- to 5-year-old children. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2010; 53 (2) 508-529
  • 49 Kefalianos E, Onslow M, Block S, Menzies R, Reilly S. Early stuttering, temperament and anxiety: two hypotheses. J Fluency Disord 2012; 37 (3) 151-163
  • 50 Conture EG . Dreams of our theoretical nights meet the realities of our empirical days: stuttering theory and research. In: Bosshardt HG, Yaruss JS, Peters HFM, eds. Fluency Disorders: Theory Research, Treatment and Self-help. Proceedings of the Third World Congress on Fluency Disorders. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Nijmegen University Press; 2001 :3–29
  • 51 Seery CH, Watkins RV, Mangelsdorf SC, Shigeto A. Subtyping stuttering II: contributions from language and temperament. J Fluency Disord 2007; 32 (3) 197-217
  • 52 Walden TA, Frankel CB, Buhr AP, Johnson KN, Conture EG, Karrass JM. Dual diathesis-stressor model of emotional and linguistic contributions to developmental stuttering. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2012; 40 (4) 633-644
  • 53 Eggers K, De Nil LF, Van den Bergh BRH. Inhibitory control in childhood stuttering. J Fluency Disord 2013; 38 (1) 1-13
  • 54 Eggers K, De Nil LF, Van den Bergh BRH. The efficiency of attentional networks in children who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2012; 55 (3) 946-959
  • 55 Johnson KN, Conture EG, Walden TA. Efficacy of attention regulation in preschool-age children who stutter: a preliminary investigation. J Commun Disord 2012; 45 (4) 263-278
  • 56 Choi D, Conture EG, Walden TA, Lambert WE, Tumanova V. Behavioral inhibition and childhood stuttering. J Fluency Disord 2013; 38 (2) 171-183
  • 57 Ntourou K, Conture EG, Walden TA. Emotional reactivity and regulation in preschool-age children who stutter. J Fluency Disord 2013; 38 (3) 260-274
  • 58 Karrass J, Walden TA, Conture EG , et al. Relation of emotional reactivity and regulation to childhood stuttering. J Commun Disord 2006; 39 (6) 402-423
  • 59 Howell P, Davis S, Patel H , et al. Fluency development and temperament in fluent children and children who stutter. In: Packman A, Meltzer A, Peters HFM, eds. Theory, Research and Therapy in Fluency Disorders: Proceedings of the 4th World Congress on Fluency Disorders. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Nijmegen University Press; 2004 :250–256. Available at: http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/103974/ . Accessed December 10, 2013
  • 60 Johnson KN, Walden TA, Conture EG, Karrass J. Spontaneous regulation of emotions in preschool children who stutter: preliminary findings. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2010; 53 (6) 1478-1495
  • 61 Eggers K, De Nil LF, Van den Bergh BR. Temperament dimensions in stuttering and typically developing children. J Fluency Disord 2010; 35 (4) 355-372
  • 62 Embrechts M, Ebben H, Franke P, van de Poel C. Temperament: a comparison between children who stutter and children who do not stutter. In: Bosshardt HG, Yaruss JS, Peters HF, , eds. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Nijmegen Press; 1998: 557-562
  • 63 Heitmann RR, Asbjørnsen A, Helland T. Attentional functions in speech fluency disorders. Logoped Phoniatr Vocol 2004; 29 (3) 119-127
  • 64 Felsenfeld S, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI. Attentional regulation in young twins with probable stuttering, high nonfluency, and typical fluency. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2010; 53 (5) 1147-1166
  • 65 Schwenk KA, Conture EG, Walden TA. Reaction to background stimulation of preschool children who do and do not stutter. J Commun Disord 2007; 40 (2) 129-141
  • 66 Anderson JD, Pellowski MW, Conture EG, Kelly EM. Temperamental characteristics of young children who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2003; 46 (5) 1221-1233
  • 67 Hagger MS, Wood C, Stiff C, Chatzisarantis NLD. Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 2010; 136 (4) 495-525
  • 68 Gailliot MT, Baumeister RF, DeWall CN , et al. Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: willpower is more than a metaphor. J Pers Soc Psychol 2007; 92 (2) 325-336
  • 69 Gailliot MT, Baumeister RF. The physiology of willpower: linking blood glucose to self-control. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2007; 11 (4) 303-327
  • 70 Lewis KE, Golberg LL. Measurements of temperament in the identification of children who stutter. Eur J Disord Commun 1997; 32 (4) 441-448
  • 71 Reilly S, Onslow M, Packman A , et al. Natural history of stuttering to 4 years of age: a prospective community-based study. Pediatrics 2013; 132 (3) 460-467
  • 72 Ortega AY, Ambrose NG. Developing physiologic stress profiles for school-age children who stutter. J Fluency Disord 2011; 36 (4) 268-273
  • 73 Van der Merwe B, Robb M, Lewis JG, Ormond T. Anxiety measures and salivary cortisol responses in preschool children who stutter. Contemp Issues Commun Sci and Disord 2011; 38: 1-10
  • 74 Arnold HS, Conture EG, Key AP, Walden T. Emotional reactivity, regulation and childhood stuttering: a behavioral and electrophysiological study. J Commun Disord 2011; 44 (3) 276-293
  • 75 Caruso AJ, Chodzko-Zajko WJ, Bidinger DA, Sommers RK. Adults who stutter: responses to cognitive stress. J Speech Hear Res 1994; 37 (4) 746-754
  • 76 Weber CM, Smith A. Autonomic correlates of stuttering and speech assessed in a range of experimental tasks. J Speech Hear Res 1990; 33 (4) 690-706
  • 77 Ellis Weismer S. Child language intervention: research issues on the horizon. In: Miller J, , ed. Research on Child Language Disorders. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1991: 233-241
  • 78 Bernstein Ratner N. Evidence-based practice in stuttering: some questions to consider. J Fluency Disord 2005; 30 (3) 163-188
  • 79 Thomas S, Chess A. Temperament: Theory and Practice. New York, NY: Brunner & Mazel; 1996
  • 80 Rothbart MK. Becoming Who We Are: Temperament and Personality in Development. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2011
  • 81 Escalona S. The Roots of Individuality: Normal Patterns of Development in Infancy. Chicago, IL: Aldine; 1968
  • 82 Conture EG. Stuttering: Its Nature, Diagnosis, and Treatment. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon; 2001
  • 83 Ellis BJ, Boyce WT. Biological sensitivity to context. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2008; 17 (3) 183-187
  • 84 Zebrowski PM, Kelly EM. Manual of Stuttering Intervention. Clifton Park, NY: Singular Publishing Group; 2002
  • 85 Lilienfeld SO. Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspect Psychol Sci 2007; 2 (1) 53-70
  • 86 Conture EG, Walden T, Arnold H, Graham C, Hartfield K, Karrass J. Communicative-emotional model of developmental stuttering. In: Ratner NB, Tetnowski J, , eds. Stuttering Research and Practice. Vol. 2: Contemporary Issues and Approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2006: 17-46
  • 87 Richels CG, Conture EG, Indirect treatment of childhood stuttering: diagnostic predictors of treatment outcome. In: Guitar B, McCauley RJ. , eds. Treatment of Stuttering: Established and Emerging Interventions. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2010