Keywords:
Sex Differences - Working Memory - Attention - Sleep
INTRODUCTION
The sleep amount in adults varies significantly among individuals, probably because
of genetic differences, although, other factors, like age and sex might underscore
this variation. Physiological changes in sleep patterns and a decrease in sleep time
are expected with aging, however, the effects of sex are less known. Some studies
have indicated that women have greater sleep latency (SL)[1] and total sleep time (TST) than men[2], especially between the age of 25 and 64 years[3]. However, independent of sex, TST short are becoming frequently in the modern civilization,
especially in the full-time workers[4].
Sleep is essential for physical and mental health, and productivity. However, due
to the constant pressure from work, social, and family activities, a large proportion
of the adult population has a total of 6 sleeping h/d or even less[4]. According to the National Sleep Foundation (NSF) young adults need around 7 to
9 hours of sleep per day[5]. A small part of the population follow the short (<5h) and long sleeper (>10h) patterns
found in adults[6]. Sleep loss leads to cognitive and behavioral performance dysfunction and deteriorates
in a dose-dependent manner[7]. There are interindividual differences in the basal sleep need and the vulnerability
to the cognitive effects of sleep loss[8]. Although it is not clear if men and women respond cognitive differently to poor
sleep quality, sex and gender differences may underlie the differential risk for sleep
disorders. Chronic difficulties initiating or maintaining sleep (insomnia) are more
frequently found in women, and are associated with anxiety and/or depressive symptoms[9].
Lapses of attention and a decline in different cognitive domains have been observed
in healthy volunteers submitted to a sleep deprivation (SD)[10] and in patients with chronic insomnia[11]. However, conflicting results have been observed, especially in relation to working
memory (WM) tests[10]-[12]. Since WM involves multicomponent cognitive processes, this outcome depends on the
specific component of the system assessed. Moreover, discrepant results may be due
to the fact that some studies did not consider age-related individual variability
and the role of sex in TST[13],[14].
Although there is evidence for sex differences in verbal and spatial memory, with
women performing better in verbal tasks and men in visuospatial tasks[15],[16], some underlying aspects need to be considered. For instance, the magnitude of sex-related
differences in the performance of verbal and spatial tasks depends on age, the tests
used and the type of ability that is measured. In several studies, the effect sizes
are small (Cohen d<0,02) or did not have significant differences[17],[18]. Moreover, the sleep time duration is not one of the variables considered in studies
that evaluate memory performance of men and women.
As there are sex differences in sleep time and in some cognitive abilities, it is
possible presume that physiological responses to SD are different in the men and women.
Some studies have suggested that women are more resilient than men to external stressors
or SD[2],[19]. Although, Rångtell et al. (2018)[20] showed that men’s WM performance remained unaffected by sleep loss after a single
night SD. In another study, one night of SD did not affected performance in different
tests of executive functions of the group of young adults formed by men and women[21]. However, as yet only a few studies have examined the role of sex in vigilances
tasks or other cognitive aspects following SD. In most articles, the results of tests
performed by men and women are evaluated together. Furthermore, sometimes the numbers
of either men or women are small, or there are other variables such as interindividual
differences and age which may confuse or mask the interpretation of the results[14],[21]-[24]. Therefore, in spite of many studies about the effects of the SD on cognition, there
are limited data on sex differences in functional consequence of impaired sleep. Moreover,
these studies are usually performed in strictly controlled laboratory settings, with
different paradigms of wake-sleep cycle, and it is therefore necessary to develop
studies in more naturalistic environments. In this study, we investigated sex differences
in quality of sleep and in cognitive abilities, and we hypothesized that impaired
sleep quality could affect the performance of men and women in attention and WM tests
differently. Furthermore, considering that sleep complaints are often associated with
depressive and/or anxiety symptoms, we evaluated these symptoms to eventually exclude
confounding factors.
MATERIAL AND METHODS
Volunteers
Volunteers were recruited from fliers, official website of University and in local
media. This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Universidade Federal
de Alagoas (CAAE: 15357913.0.0000.5013). After pre-screening by telephone, 118 individuals
(20-45 years old) signed informed consent for the study. Data of 21 were not used
for the statistical analyzes because 5 did not complete the study, 7 became ill or
took psychoactive medication during the week of the study, and 9 were excluded during
the screening for drugs[25] or cognitive decline evaluated by mini-cog test[26]. Thus, we will show the results for 50 women and 47 men non-smokers. All volunteers
met the following criteria: had ≥11 years of schooling; did not have acute medical
conditions or conclusive diagnoses of sleep/psychiatric disorders; did not have low
visual/hearing acuity; except if corrected by wearing glasses/hearing aid; did not
misuse alcohol or use medication that induces sleepiness.
Procedures
The volunteers attended the laboratory on two occasions: (1st) to complete a series of self-report measures assessing sleep quality[27],[28], circadian rhythm[29], and the use of psychoactive substances[25]. The body mass index of the volunteers was calculated based on height and weight
measurements; (2nd) the participants performed the automated WM assessment (AWMA), the D2 test, and
the psychomotor vigilance task (PVT), in this order. They also completed self-report
forms regarding symptoms of depression and anxiety[30],[31] in order to control for possible confounding factors in the performance cognitive.
During the 1st session (Monday or Tuesday), the volunteers were instructed on how to complete the
sleep diaries and use the actigraph, which they had to wear for 8-10 days. They were
told to maintain their normal daily routines and food habits, including the consumption
of caffeinated beverages throughout the experiment. However, 24 hours before the second
session they were asked to avoid ingestion of alcohol and any medication that affects
alertness or sleep. The cognitive assessment took place on the Wednesday or Thursday
of the following week, and volunteers could choose a time to attend (10h or 12h or
14h). They were instructed to choose the time of their preference, considering the
period when they were most alert. Each evaluation lasted around 2 hours, and the consumption
of stimulants beverages were prohibited for the 6 hours before the test.
Actigraphy recording
The actigraph used was the Mini Motionlogger Actigraph - Basic 32C (Ambulatory Monitoring,
Inc., Ardsley, U.S.). Data were collected using the “zero crossing mode” in one-minute
epochs. The following sleep parameters were analyzed: TST in hours; SL and wake after
sleep onset (WASO) in minutes; sleep efficiency (SE) as a percentage; sleep fragmentation
(SF); and number of awakenings (AW). The wake-sleep cycle parameters were: sleep time
(STime), end time (ETime), and midpoint of sleep time (MTime) mean and variability
(standard deviation). Moreover, information about rhythm regularity (interdaily stability),
synchronization (intradaily variability), and rhythm amplitude (relative amplitude)
were collected.
Questionnaires
Alcohol, smoking and substance involvement screening test (ASSIST): a questionnaire
to detect substance use, screening for all levels of problem or risky substance use
in adults[25]. The reliability of the instrument was good (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.80 to alcohol,
0.79 to cannabis, and 0.81 to cocaine). The cutoff adopted to select participants
was >11 points for alcohol and >4 for other substances.
Pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI): a self-rated questionnaire that assesses sleep
quality and disturbances over a 1-month period. A PSQI global score of >5 is indicative
of poor sleep quality. The seven-component scores of the Brazilian Portuguese version[27] had an overall reliability coefficient (Cronbach’s α) of 0.82.
Epworth sleepiness scale (EES): a self-report questionnaire with 8 questions that
assess the level of daytime sleepiness. An ESS score >10 suggests excessive daytime
sleepiness. The 8-item scores of the ESS in Brazilian Portuguese[28] had an overall reliability coefficient of 0.83.
The Munich chronotype questionnaire (MCTQ): a self-report questionnaire to assess
an individual’s phase of entrainment on work and work-free days. The MCTQ contains
29 questions about sleep and waking times and identifies the individual’s chronotype
based on the midpoint between sleep onset and offset on work-free days (mid-sleep).
The correlation between the Horne and Ostberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire
(MEQ) was r=-0.73 to mid-sleep times[29].
The Beck depression inventory - 2nd edition (BDI-II): a 21-item, self-report measure of the severity of depressive symptoms[30]. The suggested thresholds for levels of severity were: 0-13, minimal/no depression;
14-19, mild depression; 20-28, moderate depression; and 29-63, severe depression.
The intraclass correlation coefficient of the BDI-II was 0.89, and the Cronbach’s
alpha coefficient of internal consistency was 0.93.
The Beck anxiety inventory (BAI): a 21-item, self-report measure of the severity of
anxiety symptoms. A total score of 0-21 is considered very low anxiety, 22-35 is moderate,
and 36 or higher is severe. The reliability[31] for psychiatric samples was α=0.91, for clinical samples was α=0.86, and for non-clinical
samples α=0.86.
Cognitive tasks
The Automated Working Memory Assessment (AWMA): a computer-based standardized battery
comprising 12 tests that evaluate verbal and spatial memory[32]. The Portuguese version used was adapted by Santos and Engel (2008)[33]. The test was applied individually to each volunteer in a quiet room by a trained
technician. There are 4 subtests that evaluate verbal short-term memory (the results
given as mean composite verbal short-term memory scores/V-STM), 4 visuospatial short-term
memory (mean composite visuospatial short-term memory scores/VS-STM), 4 verbal WM
(mean composite verbal WM scores/V-WM), and 4 visuospatial WM (mean composite visuospatial
WM scores/VS-WM). The tests usually start with 3 items, with 1 item being added until
the participant is unable to recall them.
The D2 test: a neuropsychological test that evaluates sustained and selective attention[34]. The individual has 20 seconds to scan each line and mark with a cross only the
letters “d” with two dashes above or below it in any order. Are considered errors
when the number of target symbols are not marked or when the number of non-target
symbols (letter “p”) or “d” with one or three marks are marked. The measures used
were: 1) total number of characters processed (TN), the number of characters processed
before the end of each trial; 2) total correctly processed (TN-E), the total number
of characters processed minus total errors made; 3) the percent of errors (E%), the
number of errors divided by the number of characters processed.
The psychomotor vigilance task (PVT): used to test sustained vigilance and reaction
time[35]. It measures the speed with which subjects respond to a random visual stimulus over
a period of 10 minutes. The variables evaluated were: mean reaction time (RT), the
time taken to interrupt the visual stimulus; false start (FS) is the response with
a reaction time of less than 100ms and lapses are the response longer than 500ms.
Statistical procedures
Data from the actigraphic recordings were averaged over the 8 days prior to the 2nd session. The analyses of cognitive tests and behavioral scales were cross-sectional
studies obtained with the use of XLSTAT software and StatSoft version 6, considering
statistical significance to be p<0.05. To evaluate the sex effect, one-way multivariate
analyses of variance (MANOVA) were performed, using Tukey post-hoc analysis when necessary.
Cohen d effects sizes were calculated for sleep and cognitive parameters. The effects
sizes were considered positive when W>M and negative when M>W. The missing data were
due problems with the equipment or incorrect completion of the scales.
To test for associations between the variables, Spearman (ρ) or Pearson’s correlation
coefficients (r) were calculated. The sex variable was dummy coded as 1 for women
and 2 for men. If there was a significant correlation between the variables the ANCOVA
would be performed. Also, the R2 (coefficient of determination) it was calculated. The R2 indicates the % of the variability of the dependent variable which is explained by
the explanatory variables (quantitative and qualitative independent variables). The
sex was always the qualitative variable, whereas age, BDI score, and actigraphic parameters
(TST, SL, and SF) as the independent quantitative variables. To explore the relationships
among actigraphic parameters and cognitive tests by sex, we conducted partial correlations
covaried for age.
RESULTS
Demographic data
The age, education and body mass indices (BMI) data for each group are presented in
[Table 1]. No statistical differences [Wilks’ Λ=0.98, F(3, 90)=0.56, p=0.65] were found by
the one-way MANOVA between the sex for the variables of age (p=0.85), education (p=0.31),
and BMI (p=0.51).
Table 1
Demographic information and clinical characteristics by sex.
|
Men (n=47)
|
Women (n=50)
|
Age (y)
|
Education (y)
|
BMI (kg/m2)
|
Age (y)
|
Education (y)
|
BMI (kg/m2)
|
Min
|
20.0
|
12.0
|
17.4
|
20.0
|
12.0
|
15.7
|
Q1
|
23.0
|
15.0
|
23.0
|
23.0
|
15.0
|
20.3
|
Q2
|
28.0
|
16.0
|
25.8
|
27.0
|
16.0
|
24.2
|
Q3
|
38.0
|
17.0
|
27.9
|
36.0
|
17.0
|
27.9
|
Max
|
45.0
|
22.0
|
36.0
|
44.0
|
22.0
|
37.1
|
Mean
|
29.9
|
15.8
|
25.7
|
30.2
|
16.0
|
24.7
|
SD
|
8.0
|
2.3
|
3.9
|
7.7
|
1.9
|
5.1
|
Notes: First quartile (Q1), second quartile (Q2) or median, third quartile (Q3); Minimum
(Min); Maximum (Max); Standard deviation (SD); Age and education in years (y); Body
mass indices (BMI). p>0.05.
Sleep data
We did not found a significant difference in the subjective evaluation of sleep quality,
daytime sleepiness, and the chronotype (mid-sleep) between the sex [Wilks Λ=0.98,
F(3, 93)=0.58, p=0.62]. On the other hand, there were statistical difference in the
sleep-wake cycle [Wilks’ Λ=0.85, F(6, 89)=2.51, p<0.05] as sleep parameters recorded
by actigraphy [Wilks’ Λ=0.84, F(6, 90)=2.77, p<0.05]. The post-hoc tests are listed
in [Table 2]. The women reported significantly more depressive symptoms than men [Wilks’ Λ=0.92,
F(2, 94)=3.61, p=0.03].
Table 2
Sleep quality, daytime sleepiness and mid-sleep by MCTQ; behavioral scales; sleep-wake
cycle and sleep parameters by sex.
|
Men (n=47)
|
|
Women (n=50)
|
|
|
|
Min
|
Max
|
Mean
|
SD
|
Min
|
Max
|
Mean
|
SD
|
d
|
p
|
Subjective evaluation
|
PSQI
|
0
|
13
|
6.2
|
2.7
|
3.0
|
12.0
|
6.7
|
2.2
|
0.20
|
0.3
|
Epworth
|
2
|
21
|
9.6
|
3.9
|
2.0
|
22.0
|
10.2
|
4.6
|
0.14
|
0.5
|
MCTQ
|
1.2
|
7.0
|
4.1
|
1.2
|
1.1
|
7.7
|
3.9
|
1.5
|
-0.15
|
0.6
|
Behavioral scales
|
BAI
|
0
|
30
|
8.3
|
6.6
|
2.0
|
24.0
|
8.6
|
5.8
|
0.05
|
0.8
|
BDI
|
0
|
25
|
9.2
|
7.1
|
1.0
|
26.0
|
12.5
|
7.3
|
0.46
|
<0.05
|
Sleep-wake cycle by actigraphy
|
STIME
|
21.5
|
27.0
|
24.3
|
1.1
|
21.2
|
26.2
|
23.7
|
1.06
|
-0.56
|
<0.01
|
ETIME
|
5.4
|
11.0
|
7.2
|
1.2
|
5.3
|
10.5
|
7.07
|
1.13
|
-0.11
|
0.5
|
MPOINT
|
1.45
|
7.1
|
3.8
|
1.1
|
1.9
|
6.2
|
3.38
|
0.98
|
-0.40
|
0.06
|
IS
|
0.10
|
0.82
|
0.54
|
0.18
|
0.33
|
0.81
|
0.60
|
0.11
|
0.41
|
<0.05
|
IV
|
0.34
|
0.84
|
0.52
|
0.12
|
0.35
|
0.70
|
0.53
|
0.08
|
0.10
|
0.8
|
RA
|
0.51
|
0.94
|
0.79
|
0.09
|
0.44
|
0.94
|
0.83
|
0.09
|
0.44
|
<0.05
|
Sleep parameters
by actigraphy
|
TST
|
3.8
|
8.0
|
5.8
|
0.8
|
2.1
|
8.4
|
6.2
|
1.0
|
0.53
|
<0.05
|
SL
|
5.3
|
57.4
|
16.3
|
10.9
|
3.2
|
61.9
|
21.7
|
14.3
|
0.42
|
<0.05
|
SE
|
64.5
|
96.1
|
87.2
|
7.3
|
48.9
|
97.2
|
89.2
|
8.3
|
0.26
|
0.2
|
WASO
|
15.8
|
160.6
|
51.7
|
32.6
|
9.0
|
129.2
|
44.1
|
29.5
|
-0.25
|
0.2
|
SF
|
2.1
|
10.2
|
4.6
|
2.0
|
1.2
|
9.0
|
3.8
|
1.8
|
-0.41
|
<0.05
|
AW
|
6.8
|
30.8
|
15.1.
|
5.8
|
21.2
|
26.2
|
13.3
|
5.6
|
-0.31
|
0.1
|
Notes: MANOVA, followed by a posteriori Tukey. Cohen’s d effects sizes were considered
positive when W>M and negative when M>W. Minimum (Min); Maximum (Max); Standard deviation
(SD). Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), Munich Chronotype Questionnaire (MCTQ.
Number of volunteers (N); Sleep Time (STime), End time (ETime), Midpoint of sleep
time (MTime). Interdaily Stability (IS), Intradaily Variability (IV), Relative Amplitude
(RA).Total Sleep Time (TST) in hours; Sleep Latency (SL) and Wake After Sleep Onset
(WASO) in minutes; Sleep Efficiency (SE) in percentage; Sleep fragmentation (SF),
Awakenings number (AW).
Correlation analyses between the variables
Statistically significant correlation data between the variables are summarized in
the [Table 3] and the [Table 4]. [Table 5] displays the significant partial relationship between actigraphic parameters and
cognitive tests separated by sex while controlling for age. Only parameters statistically
significant were shown in the [Tables 4] and [5].
Table 3
Correlation coefficients between the demographic variables, BAI and BDI scores and
sleep variables.
|
Sex
|
Age
|
Education (Y)
|
BAI
|
BDI
|
Actigraphic parameters:
STIME
ETIME
MPOINT
TST
SL
WASO
SE-SD
SF
AW
|
0.30 -- 0.22 -0.24 -- -- -- 0.21 --
|
-0.42 -0.30 -0.39 -- -- -- -- -- --
|
-- -- -- 0.21 -- -- -- -- --
|
-- -- -- -- -- 0.23 0.23 0.22 0.26
|
-- -- -- -- 0.26 -- -- -- --
|
Subjective sleep evaluation:
PSQI global score
Epworth score
MCTQ (mid-sleep)
|
-- -- --
|
-- -0.21 -0.47
|
-- -0.33 -0.21
|
0.23 0.27 --
|
0.27 -- --
|
Notes: All correlation coefficients are statistically significant (p<0.05); Sleep
time (STime); End time (ETime); Midpoint of sleep time (MTime); Total sleep time (TST);
Sleep latency (SL); Wake after sleep onset (WASO); Sleep efficiency (SE); Sleep efficiency
standard-deviation (SE-SD); Sleep fragmentation (SF); Number of awakenings (AW); Beck
anxiety inventory (BAI); Beck depression inventory (BDI); Pittsburgh sleep quality
index (PSQI); Munich chronotype questionnaire (MCTQ).
Table 4
Correlation coefficients between the demographic variables, BAI, and BDI scores, sleep
variables and cognitive tests.
|
AWMA (N=96)
|
|
PVT (N=86)
|
V- STM
|
VS-STM
|
V- WM
|
VS- WM
|
D2-TEST (N=94)
|
RT
|
FS
|
Lapses
|
TN
|
TN-E
|
E%
|
Sex
|
--
|
0.38
|
0.37
|
0.43
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
-0.32
|
0.32
|
--
|
Age
|
-0.30
|
-0.26
|
-0.30
|
-0.21
|
-0.41
|
-0.41
|
0.36
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
BAI
|
--
|
--
|
-0.20
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
0.32
|
--
|
0.30
|
BDI
|
--
|
--
|
-0.22
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
0.36
|
--
|
0.33
|
MCQT
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
0.25
|
0.23
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
EPWORT
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
0.25
|
--
|
0.25
|
STIME
|
--
|
0.25
|
--
|
0.24
|
0.24
|
0.21
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
ETIME
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
0.23
|
--
|
SL
|
-0.23
|
--
|
-0.24
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
0.31
|
--
|
0.22
|
SE-SD
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
-0.22
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
Notes: All correlation coefficients are statistically significant (p<0.05); Sleep
time (STime); End time (ETime); Sleep latency (SL); Sleep efficiency standard deviation
(SE-SD); Sleep fragmentation (SF); Number of awakenings (AW); Beck anxiety inventory
(BAI); Beck depression inventory (BDI); Munich chronotype questionnaire (MCTQ); Automated
working memory assessment (AWMA): mean composites of verbal short-term memory scores
(V-STM), visuospatial short-term memory scores (VS-STM), verbal working memory scores
(V-WM), and visuospatialworking memory scores (VS-WM); D2 test: total number of characters
processed (TN), total correctly processed (TN-E), and percent of errors (E%); Psychomotor
vigilance task (PVT): reaction time (RT); false start (FS).
Table 5
Partial correlations, controlling age, between cognitive tests, and actigraphic parameters
separated by sex.
WOMEN
|
TST
|
SL
|
SE
|
WASO
|
SE-DP
|
STIME
|
ETIME
|
MIDPOINT
|
BAI
|
TN
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
*
|
*
|
*
|
--
|
TN-E
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
*
|
0.29
|
0.29
|
--
|
E%
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
*
|
-0.33
|
-0.29
|
--
|
MEN
|
TST
|
SL
|
SE
|
WASO
|
SE-DP
|
STIME
|
ETIME
|
MIDPOINT
|
BAI
|
TN
|
--
|
-0.47#
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
TN-E
|
--
|
-0.46#
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
E%
|
--
|
0.52#
|
--
|
*
|
--
|
--
|
0.34
|
--
|
0.31
|
V-WM
|
--
|
*
|
*
|
*
|
*
|
-0.38#
|
-0.53#
|
-0.50#
|
-0.36
|
VS-WM
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
-0.42#
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
--
|
Notes: All correlation coefficients are statistically significant (p<0.05); SF and
AW not showed any significant correlation;
*When not controlling for age, this relationship becomes significant (p<0.05); #When
controlling for anxiety symptom, this relationship also maintain statistical significance
(p<0.05).
3.4. Cognitive tasks
The results of the sustained attention D2-test, the four composites of AWMA and PVT
parameters of the women and men are shown in [Table 5]. There were a significant main effect of group for AWMA scores [Wilks’ Λ=0.77, F(4,
92)=6.61, p<0.01] and PVT parameters [Wilks’ Λ=0.79, F(3, 82)=6.91, p<0.01]. The post
hoc difference between the groups are shown in the [Table 6]. The results of D2-test did not show differences between the groups [Wilks’ Λ=0.97,
F(3, 90)=0.75, p=0.52] ([Table 6]).
Table 6
Scores of cognitive tasks by sex.
|
WOMEN
|
MEN
|
d
|
p-value
|
R2
|
AWMA
V-STM
VS-STM
V-WM
VS-WM
|
(n=50)
26.0± 3.4 27.9± 3.1 19.0± 2.7 20.8± 4.2
|
(n=47)
26.9± 3.4 30.9± 4.0* 21.6± 3.7* 25.3± 5.3*
|
-0.35 -0.78 -0.61 -084
|
0.17a, (0.14)
b
<0.01a,b
<0.01a,b
<0.01a,b
|
16% 23% 29% 26%
|
D2 TEST
TN
TN-E
E%
|
(n=49)
474.4± 68.1 343.5± 98.3 28.9± 11.5
|
(n=45)
473.8± 83.2 349.0± 117.8 28.4± 13.1
|
0.39 0.35 12.8
|
0.97
a
(0.96)
b
0.80
a
(0.79)
b
0.83
a
(0.81)
b
|
22% 21% 18%
|
PVT TEST
RT
FS
Lapses
|
(n=49)
296.2± 56.8 0.6± 1.0 2.6± 6.3
|
(n=37)
265.0± 24.7* 3.0±5.2* 1.0± 1.0
|
0.69 -0.85 0.43
|
<0.02a,b
<0.01a,b
0.13
a
(0.11)
b
|
23% 18% 15%
|
Notes:
aMANOVA;
bANCOVA adjusted for age, BDI, TST, SL, and SF; Cohen’s d effects sizes were considered
positive when W>M and negative when M>W; R2 considering age, BDI, TST, SL, and SF;
Automated working memory assessment (AWMA): Mean composites of verbal short-term memory
scores (V-STM); Visuospatial short-term memory scores (VS-STM); Verbal working memory
scores (V-WM); and Visuospatial working memory scores (VS-WM); D2 test: total number
of characters processed (TN); Total correctly processed (TN-E); Percent of errors
(E%); Psychomotor vigilance task (PVT): reaction time (RT); false start (FS).
Given the R2 value, 14% of the variability of the variable V-STM is explained by the covariates
([Table 6]). Based on the type III sum of squares, the age (p<0.01) and SL (p<0.05) brought
significant information to explain the variability of the V-STM. In relation to VS-STM,
23% of the variability is explained by the covariates age (p<0.01) and sex (p<0.001)
being the last was the most influential.
The results of WM tests showed that the 29% and 26% of the variability of the dependent
variables V-WM and VS-WM were explained by the explanatory variables age (p<0.01)
and sex (p<0.01), age was the most influential to V-WM and sex to VS-WM.
Considering the same predictors (age, sex, LS, TST, and SF) the R2 value of D2-test parameters it was around 20% (see [Table 6]) in that the covariate age (p<0.01) the covariate that brought significant information
to explain the variability of the TN, TN-E, and TE%.
Given the R2 value, 23%, 18%, and 15% of the variability of the dependent variables RT, FS, and
lapse, respectively, were explained by the covariates. The covariate sex (p<0.05)
brought significant information for RT and FS, whereas BDI score (p<0.05) it was significant
for RT and lapse parameters. The covariate TST (p<0.05) brought significant information
only to FS parameter.
DISCUSSION
The main objective of this research was to evaluate the interaction between sleep
quality and sex on cognitive performance. In addition, we evaluated the relation of
these variables with depression and anxiety. Our results suggest that impaired sleep
is associated with cognitive deficits and feelings of anxiety and depression, with
men presenting more pronounced cognitive impairments.
Our findings are consistent with previous literature showing that women typically
have longer and less-fragmented sleep than men. It is not clear if the biological
need for sleep is different between women and men, although, sex differences have
recently been reported in sleep habits and preferences[36]. Women spend more time in bed, and seem to have more complaints about poor sleep
quality than men, although this perception is not reflected by objective measures
of sleep pattern[37]. They sleep objectively better than men, presenting a lower percentage of stage
1, and a higher percentage of slow wave sleep[2],[37]. Mong and Cusmano (2016)[38] described sex differences in various aspects of sleep of animals and humans. They
suggested that the sex differences are predominantly incurring to the effects of ovarian
sex steroids in females, and that would not be different in humans.
In addition, our results revealed that women present discreet, yet significantly higher,
relative amplitude and interdaily stability than men. This suggests that women perform
more motor activities during the day and/or low movements during the night and have
a greater regularity of wake-sleep rhythm. Furthermore, women displayed earlier bedtimes
and sleep midpoints than men, despite this difference not being observed in the subjective
reports. These sex-dependent disparities in bedtime have previously been described
in college students[39] and have also been shown to be independent of both marital status and whether a
child is at home[40]. Cain et al. (2010)[41] reported that women have an earlier onset of melatonin peak levels and a higher
melatonin amplitude than men. This group also showed that a morning preference is
associated with a shorter intrinsic circadian period and is described more in women
than in men[42].
Poor sleep quality was associated with worse performance in the visual sustained attention
and memory tests, and more complaints of anxiety and depression. Anxiety results in
attentional resources being allocated to irrelevant distractors thereby impairing
attentional control, and consequently affecting the process of memory[43]. Visuospatial ability was more resistant to the effect of impaired sleep quality,
and there was a smaller decline in V-STM. However, there were moderate degree correlation
between VS-WM and irregular SE. Remarkably, in our study when the men and women were
analyzed separately, the correlation becomes significant only in the men group. The
visual focused attention impairment was maintained even when controlling the anxiety
scores. The main complaints of poor sleepers in our study were difficulty in falling
asleep or maintaining sleep during the night, still that it is not possible to evaluate
the depth of sleep without polysomnography. However, despite men having less sleep
than women, the main sleep parameter associated with low scores was SL. Interestingly,
we also found that later bedtime and wake times were associated with low cognitive
test scores in men. In fact, the sleep onset time of the men was after midnight, and
the combination of circadian misalignment and chronic sleep loss may have contributed
to cognitive impairment.
Although chronic insomnia has an important socioeconomic impact, the cognitive deficit
mechanisms underlying sleep loss remain poorly understood. Studies with healthy volunteers
submitted to different SD paradigms and fMRI records of patients with insomnia have
shown hyper- or hypofunction in several structures during the performance of memory
tests[44]. These differences seem to depend on the level of relative cognitive demand required
to complete specific tasks. Even so, interindividual differences and the role of sex,
although poorly explored, may contribute to the inconsistent findings derived from
functional neuroimaging studies.
The PVT was not affected by poor sleep quality; however, there was sex effect in our
study. Pejovic et al. (2013)[45] observed a tendency for slower reaction times in volunteers who had their sleep
reduced from 8 to 6 hours/night for one week. In this study, no sex effects were observed,
however, slow wave sleep at baseline was associated with less subjective sleepiness
during the restriction period and greater amelioration after recovery in women. This
study corroborate previous result[46] that suggest men are more affected due to significant increases in inflammatory
markers after mild sleep restriction, which are not seen in women.
Distinctive cognitive performance was observed in men and women. Our results are in
agreement with the literature that men perform better in visuospatial tests[15]. There is some evidence that these differences between men and women can be increased
by limiting the execution time during visuospatial tasks, whereas untimed tests allow
women to demonstrate their abilities better[47]. Unlike some studies, a better performance of women in verbal tasks was not observed[15],[16]. There was no difference in V-STM and the mean performance of men was slightly better
than women with respect to V-WM. However, the differences between sexes in verbal
tasks are frequently small, do not consistently favor females and depend on the type
of verbal ability involved[47]. Studies addressing the sex difference in the performance cognitive has proposed
that fluctuation of hormones during menstrual cycle influence in the memory processing[37]. However, studies on menstrual cycle-dependent changes in cognition have yielded
inconsistent results[48]. Nevertheless, in our study was not controlled the menstrual cycle phase.
Further differences were observed when analyzing other cognitive abilities. Sex predicted
shorter reaction times, while controlling for depressive and anxiety symptoms. Although
volunteers were instructed to be “as fast as possible” at the PVT, men had shorter
reaction times (~ 30ms difference) than women. However, they made more mistakes, frequently
pressing the button without seeing the stimuli. Blatter et al. (2006)[49] suggested that women, despite showing slower reaction times, maintain accuracy because
they avoid impulsive responses. In terms of sustained visual attention, no difference
was observed between men and women, suggesting that the sex effect in the PVT is not
due to differences in visual attention. In fact, PVT performance improved significantly
in women after a military training program[50] suggesting that sex differences may be due to distinctive cognitive strategies chosen
by men and women[51].
Our results suggest that sleep disturbances may impose different cognitive impacts
on men and women. Considering the sex differences in sleep patterns, separate sex-specific
analyses could provide additional information on the relationship between difficulties
in falling asleep and cognitive impairments. Women tend to take longer to sleep, while
men appear to sleep a little less than women. These findings may explain the confusion
encountered by some studies in interpreting results, especially regarding initial
insomnia. In addition, differences in cognitive performance between the sexes, even
in the absence of sleep complaints, may add further bias in our understanding of the
interaction between insufficient sleep and cognition. Therefore, sex differences in
sleep quality and cognitive skills should be taken into account in future research
in this field.
Limitations
There are some limitations in our study. Clinical exams were not done. In order to
control for the presence of sleep disturbances or pathological cognitive decline,
several scales were applied during the interview to exclude people with any symptoms
of these conditions. Moreover, most of the volunteers had an annual medical checkup
and reported being in good physical condition. Another limitation is the fact that
actigraphy does not perfectly measure sleep hours when compared with the gold standard
of polysomnography. In addition, it does not allow the analysis of sleep architecture.
Actigraphy may overestimate SL, TST, and SE, while underestimating intermittent awakenings[52]. Despite these limitations, actigraphy provides valuable information about the wake-sleep
cycle because it allows longitudinal studies to be performed without interfering with
the routine of the individual. Furthermore, it permits patients with insomnia to be
objectively differentiated from normal sleepers[53].
CONCLUSION
The sex-related differences observed in this study highlight the need to separate
women and men in studies that evaluate the effect of sleep deprivation or sleep disorder
on cognitive function. This is particularly important due to sex differences in sleep
pattern, cognitive test performance, and reactions to external stressors.
Abbreviations list:
Alcohol, smoking and substance involvement screening test (ASSIST)
Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA)
Automated working memory assessment (AWMA)
Awakenings (AW)
Beck anxiety inventory (BAI)
Beck depression inventory - 2nd edition (BDI-II)
Body mass indices (BMI)
End time (ETime)
False start (FS)
Interdaily stability (IS)
Intradaily variability (IV)
Maximum (Max)
Mean (m)
Midpoint of sleep time (MTime)
Minimum (Min)
Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)
Munich chronotype questionnaire (MCTQ)
Number of volunteers (N)
Percent of errors (E%)
Pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI)
Psychomotor vigilance tests (PVT)
Reaction time (RT)
Relative amplitude (RA)
Sleep deprivation (SD)
Sleep efficiency (SE)
Sleep efficiency standard deviation (SE-SD)
Sleep fragmentation (SF)
Sleep latency (SL)
Sleep time (STime)
Standard deviation (SD)
Total correctly processed (TN-E)
Total number (TN)
Total sleep time (TST)
Verbal short-term memory (V-STM)
Verbal working memory (V-WM)
Visuospatial short-term memory (VS-STM)
Visuospatial working memory (VS-WM)
Wake after sleep onset (WASO)
Wilks lambda (Wilks’ Λ)
Working memory (WM)
Years (y)