J Am Acad Audiol 2017; 28(03): 232-247
DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.15123
Tutorial
Thieme Medical Publishers 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA.

Pediatric Minimum Speech Test Battery

Kristin Uhler
*   University of Colorado Denver School of Medicine, Aurora, CO
,
Andrea Warner-Czyz
†   The University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX
,
Rene Gifford
‡   Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN
,
PMSTB Working Group
§   A complete list of the PMSTB Working Group members appears on page 245.
› Author Affiliations
Further Information

Corresponding author

Rene Gifford
Vanderbilt University
Nashville, TN 37232

Publication History

Publication Date:
26 June 2020 (online)

 

Abstract

Background:

Assessment of patient outcomes and documentation of treatment efficacy serves as an essential component of (re)habilitative audiology; however, no standardized protocol exists for the assessment of speech perception abilities for children with hearing loss. This presents a significant challenge in tracking performance of children who utilize various hearing technologies for within-subjects assessment, between-subjects assessment, and even across different facilities.

Purpose:

The adoption and adherence to a standardized assessment protocol could help facilitate continuity of care, assist in clinical decision making, allow clinicians and researchers to define benchmarks for an aggregate clinical population, and in time, aid with patient counseling regarding expectations and predictions regarding longitudinal outcomes.

Design:

The Pediatric Minimum Speech Test Battery (PMSTB) working group—comprised of clinicians, scientists, and industry representatives—commenced in 2012 and has worked collaboratively to construct the first PMSTB, which is described here.

Conclusions:

Implementation of the PMSTB in clinical practice and dissemination of associated data are both critical for achieving the next level of success for children with hearing loss and for elevating pediatric hearing health care ensuring evidence-based practice for (re)habilitative audiology.


#

INTRODUCTION

Nearly 5 yr ago, [Uhler and Gifford (2014)] conducted a nationwide survey of pediatric audiologists in an attempt to characterize common clinical practices and protocols. This survey was distributed to 700 audiologists attending the 2012 American Cochlear Implant Alliance meeting via a pencil-and-paper questionnaire as well as to 375 audiologists via Research Electronic Data Capture ([Harris et al, 2009]). Results revealed a wide variety of tests, implementations, and protocols across facilities, highlighting the need to standardize a speech test battery to monitor outcomes in children with hearing loss. [Uhler and Gifford (2014)] presented these results at the 2013 AAA AudiologyNow! Conference in Anaheim, CA, and later that year at the 2013 American Cochlear Implant Alliance symposium in Washington, DC. Attendees at these meetings plus the original 375 pediatric audiologists emailed in 2012 were again invited to participate, via Research Electronic Data Capture, in the development of a standardized test battery. The result of these efforts was the Pediatric Minimum Speech Test Battery (PMSTB) working group. This working group was comprised of a heterogeneous group of academic, clinical, research, and industry professionals—all with pediatric audiology experience—determined to find consensus on best practices for the assessment of speech understanding in children with hearing loss. The underlying goal of this working group was to develop and disseminate a consensus document that would be both data driven and aligned with current clinical practices (i.e., avoiding research tools not validated in large clinical populations). Therefore, this PMSTB embodies the three cornerstones of evidence-based practice: research, clinical expertise, and patient/family concerns.

The PMSTB working group held two conference calls (November 22, 2013, and March 21, 2014) and created a publicly available wiki page to post meeting minutes and share center protocols (https://sites.google.com/site/pediatricmstbworkinggroup/home). During these conference calls, the working group members were encouraged to recruit additional colleagues for participation and to provide feedback on the materials under development. Once the pediatric working group agreed upon a protocol on March 21, 2014, the primary authors wrote the manual, posted it on the project wiki, and requested further feedback. Thus, this first implemented version of the PMSTB integrated feedback from scientists, clinicians, and industry professionals, resulting in the manual presented in [Supplemental Appendix S1], supplemental to the online version of this article.

There were numerous factors motivating the development of the PMSTB including the potential scientific and clinical benefits afforded by a standardized PMSTB as well as our professional obligation to monitor a child’s auditory progress and to maximize his or her auditory potential. The PMSTB battery includes measures designed to evaluate speech discrimination for infants and word/sentence recognition for children using hearing technology before entering school (Appendix A). Please note that the PMSTB emphasizes the use of developmentally appropriate measures consistent with a child’s language skills before school entry. At any time that a child’s skills demonstrate readiness to transition to a more challenging measure than those included here, clinicians are encouraged to consider the adult Minimum Speech Test Battery ([MSTB, 2011]).

As stated above, no standardized protocol currently exists to assess speech perception abilities for children with hearing loss. This presents a significant challenge in tracking performance of children who use various hearing technologies (e.g., hearing aids [HA], cochlear implants [CIs], osseointegrated devices, frequency modulation [FM], or digital modulation technology) within the same child, across different children, and across different facilities. Children with hearing loss represent a heterogeneous population, making the generalization of outcomes a challenge. For this reason, large sample sizes are essential to establishing consistent and standardized reporting of outcomes and to address the variance in performance. Studies completed at single sites in both HA (e.g., [Stelmachowicz et al, 2010]; [Leibold et al, 2013]; [Hillock-Dunn et al, 2014]; [McCreery et al, 2014]; [Hillock-Dunn et al, 2015]) and CI users (e.g., [Desai et al, 2008]; [Sarant et al, 2009]) provide highly valuable information contributing to the body of knowledge in our field; however, variability in study protocols (i.e., assessment measures, presentation levels, presentation method, sampled ages) across the different studies/centers compromises generalization to the larger clinical population.

Transitioning to a uniform test battery, similar to the adult [MSTB (2011)], can afford greater consistency in testing as well as greater ability to pool data and generalize findings. Specifically, the PMSTB may help accomplish several large-scale goals, as outlined below:

  1. Setting guidelines and performance level across sites. The development and implementation of a uniform test battery can foster collaboration and compilation of information across individual centers. A standardized test battery would provide a much-needed guideline for the assessment of speech perception abilities in infants and young children in both the clinic and the research laboratory. For example, implementation of this battery and subsequent publication of outcome data for the same measures across multiple centers and research teams will provide us with valuable age normative data for various degrees of hearing loss, ages, and interventions. Availability of these normative data in the peer-reviewed literature will allow us to track progress of our own patient population for a given center as well as afford comparison across institutions, interventions, educational approaches, and other patient-specific variables not currently possible.

  2. Setting realistic expectations for families. The widespread adoption of a standardized test battery will yield outcomes that can facilitate family counseling regarding realistic expectations for speech perception abilities.

    • a. Establishing expected outcomes allows comparison by chronologic age, device experience, developmental age, language ability, hearing modality (e.g., unilateral versus bilateral versus. bimodal), etc. to identify children not meeting expected benchmarks.

    • b. A standardized test battery addressing the evaluation of “all children with hearing loss”—including children with secondary disabilities—will provide us with the necessary information to identify children who may require additional services and intervention. Many studies exclude children with secondary disabilities, a group that constitutes 15–47% ([Eze et al, 2013]; [Inscoe and Bones, 2016]) of children with hearing loss ([Yoshinaga-Itano et al, 1998]). Excluding these children from assessment in both the clinic and research laboratory works as a disservice to the field in two ways. First, some children with secondary issues can complete speech perception tasks. For example, [Eshraghi et al (2015)] found that two-thirds of children with autism spectrum disorder using CIs can at least identify or recognize simple phrases, an early-developing auditory skill, per parent report. Second, eliminating children with secondary disabilities from research studies hinders our ability as clinicians to identify suitable expectations and appropriate recommendations for supplemental technology (such as home FM use) and outcomes assessment in these children. However, implementing a standardized PMSTB will allow specification of expected progress based not only on presence or absence of additional special needs, but also (eventually) expected progress by specific type of additional special needs (e.g., autism, cerebral palsy).

  • 3. Guiding clinical decision-making. Availability and use of a standardized test battery can provide great value in clinical decision-making regarding the need for additional CI and/or HA programming, assistive technology (FM or DM), bilateral CI candidacy, or some combination thereof. Establishment of standardized measures to evaluate children with hearing loss will allow clinicians and researchers to evaluate markers for on-target versus slower progress. For example, inability to perform within one standard deviation of peers with similar degree of hearing loss may indicate time for a change in technology or recommended therapy to optimize a child’s outcomes.

  • 4. Supporting a database registry of children with hearing loss. There has been considerable discussion among professionals and policymakers regarding the move toward a national or international registry of CI recipient outcomes—something already in place for CI recipients in France and Switzerland ([Brand et al, 2014]) and in the process of development for adult CI recipients in the United States ([Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services, 2004]). Having a standardized battery for pediatric speech perception is of critical importance if we are to work toward this goal. The purposes for a national registry of any given intervention or etiology include

    • a. elevation of clinical practice through standardized protocols and assessment batteries;

    • b. implementation of evidence-based practice and the subsequent study of the outcomes on an aggregate population;

    • c. verification and validation of the recommended treatment (in this case, cochlear implantation); and

    • d. development of a virtual network of clinicians and researchers allowing for a free exchange of data and experience. In the absence of a standardized assessment battery, implementation of a data registry would be essentially useless as it would be nearly impossible to summarize the effectiveness of a particular intervention and to pool data across sites and even across clinicians within a given institution.

The adoption of the PMSTB with a hierarchical protocol will allow for consistency of assessment methods across clinicians and sites. As mentioned above, this alone will facilitate the collection and dissemination of large-scale normative datasets and auditory milestones for common speech recognition metrics administered to children with hearing loss ([Uhler and Gifford, 2014]). While the release of the PMSTB will not automatically result in multicenter studies nor in the development of a pediatric registry, without it, such endeavors would be nearly impossible. Using the adult CI population as a comparison, since the release of the adult MSTB in 2011, researchers have published 11 peer-reviewed papers describing outcomes for adult CI recipients using AzBio sentence lists ([Dorman et al, 2012]; [Gifford et al, 2014]; [Koch et al, 2014]; [Mahmoud and Ruckenstein, 2014]; [Massa and Ruckenstein, 2014]; [Dorman et al, 2015]; [Wolfe et al, 2015]; [Beyea et al, 2016]; [Olds et al, 2016]; [Roland et al, 2016]; [Runge et al, 2016]). These 11 papers all included “at least” 30 participants with CIs (M = 69 participants; range 32–125) and met classification criteria as a Quality-B or higher study per the quality assessment grading metrics employed by the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Methods Guide for Comparative Effectiveness Reviews ([AHRQ, 2011]; [2014]). Only two peer-reviewed publications meeting these criteria existed before the release of the adult MSTB in 2011 ([Spahr et al, 2007]; [Gifford et al, 2008]). Thus, historical precedent in the peer-reviewed literature supports the adoption of a uniform test battery and the subsequent dissemination of associated data.


#

HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED TEST BATTERY

This working group is not the first to attempt construction of a standardized and uniform test battery for assessing speech understanding in the pediatric audiology clinic. Historically, several have attempted to develop a standardized test battery for use with pediatric CI recipients ([Tyler et al, 1986]; [1987]; [Eisenberg et al, 2006]). During the US investigation of the safety and efficacy of pediatric cochlear implantation in the 1980s, several published reports described the types of speech perception metrics recommended for use in this population. Though a detailed protocol outlining “specific measures” was neither recommended nor universally adopted, these pioneering efforts resulted in an agreement regarding the “minimal acceptable characteristics” of the stimuli included in such a battery. Those characteristics include

  • an ability to gauge conversational abilities;

  • a capacity to meet developmental language and cognitive abilities of the child;

  • consistency of testing (i.e., high test–retest reliability);

  • availability of multiple equivalent lists to avoid familiarity of test materials;

  • standardization of recordings to avoid monitored live voice (MLV) presentation; and

  • a variety of measures (e.g., words, sentences, nonlinguistic; [Tyler et al, 1986]; [1987]; [Waltzman et al, 1990]; [Osberger et al, 1991]).

Other researchers have also developed a hierarchical protocol for assessing speech recognition abilities in children with hearing loss. The Childhood Development after Cochlear Implantation (CDaCI) investigative team launched the first longitudinal multicenter investigation of various outcomes following pediatric cochlear implantation ([Eisenberg et al, 2006]; [Fink et al, 2007]; [Niparko et al, 2010]). The CDaCI investigative team defined a uniform hierarchical protocol to meet the minimum requirements listed above for a chosen set of speech perception measures. The PMSTB protocol in this manuscript builds on concepts initiated by the CDaCI Investigative team by incorporating measures more commonly used in audiology clinics and newly developed and validated materials (e.g., Pediatric AzBio) that have emerged since the CDaCI project officially launched in early 2001.

The PMSTB introduces measures in a hierarchical organization of task difficulty allowing us to track a child’s progress over time—similar to its predecessors such as the CDaCI study protocol ([Eisenberg et al, 2006]; [Wang et al, 2008]; [Niparko et al, 2010]), the Longitudinal Outcomes of Children with Hearing Impairment (LOCHI; [Ching et al, 2013]) and the Outcomes of Children with Hearing Loss study (OCHL; [Tomblin et al, 2014]; [2015] [McCreery et al, 2015]). [Table 1] summarizes the speech perception measures selected for the PMSTB. Selected measures were restricted to those that were both clinically available for purchase as well as validated at the time of PMSTB consensus (2013–2015). Many of the measures included in the PMSTB are consistent with measures used in larger pediatric research studies (e.g., CDACI study [[Eisenberg et al, 2006]; [Wang et al, 2008]; [Niparko et al, 2010]], LOCHI study [[Ching et al, 2013]], OCHL study [[McCreery et al, 2015]]). As with all three established protocols (CDaCI, LOCHI, and OCHL), the proposed PMSTB also incorporates parent questionnaires to address outcomes in a preverbal population rather than to assess speech perception. Therefore, we provide a more thorough description regarding appropriate populations and implementation of questionnaires in [Supplemental Appendix S1].

Table 1

Rationale for Test Selection

Tests Selected

Open or Closed Set

Stimulus

Listening Condition

Norming Population

Pros

Cons

VRISD

Closed

Syllable

Quiet

Normal hearing

Independent of language abilities

Requires conditioned head turn; norms required for additional contrasts

ESP, Low Verbal or Standard

Closed

Word

Quiet

Hearing loss

Assesses an array of speech discrimination abilities

Toys can be distracting

PSI

Closed

Word, sentence

Quiet and noise

Normal hearing and hearing loss

Can be done in presence of semantic distractor

Limited number of lists

MLNT/LNT

Open

Word

Quiet

N/A

Familiar words for children with limited vocabulary, lists with varying lexical difficulty

Limited number of lists, norms needed

CNC

Open

Word

Quiet

N/A

Use of prompt “ready,” included in adult MSTB

50-word list is most reliable, child may need breaks

BKB

Open

Sentence

Quiet

N/A

Use of prompt “ready,” low context for younger children

Norms not available for children younger than 5 yr

BKB in SIN

Open

Sentence

Noise

Normal hearing

Adaptive test, norms across life span

Norms not available for children younger than 5 yr ([Schafer, 2010])

Pediatric AzBio (BabyBio)

Open

Sentence

Quiet and noise

Normal hearing

Norms for children 5–12 yr, equivalent lists

16 lists, female talker only

Notes: Tests selected by the PMSTB working group. This table describes whether the tests are open or closed set, stimulus type and the listening conditions that can be assessed, norming population, as well as pros and cons for each test. LNT = Lexical Neighborhood Test; MLNT = Multisyllabic Lexical Neighborhood Test; N/A = not applicable; SIN = Speech-in-Noise.


This first iteration of the PMSTB focuses on infants and young children before school entrance at the age of 5 yr or a language equivalent of 5 yr. The working group encourages the administration of multiple types of tests (e.g., word and sentence measures) per testing session, but recognizes that attaining a complete battery will likely require multiple sessions. Thus, the speech perception data obtained from this hierarchical PMSTB protocol will evolve over time as the child’s developmental and language abilities mature. Additionally, we recognize that some of the PMSTB measures may have a limited number of validated lists, in some cases precluding assessment of all listening conditions in a single test session. For this reason, the PMSTB working group fully supports the development and subsequent validation of additional measures for assessment of speech understanding in preschool- and school-aged populations as well as future modifications to the measures recommended in the PMSTB hierarchy.

The PMSTB guidelines, summarized in Table 2 of [Supplemental Appendix S1], highlight testing at multiple intensities (i.e., conversational speech level in quiet [60 dBA], conversational speech level in noise [65 dBA], soft speech level [50 dBA]) in multiple listening environments (i.e., quiet, noise, +5 dB SNR) using a ranked array of speech stimuli (i.e., phonemes, words, and sentences). Multiple studies have assessed outcomes for individuals with hearing loss at these levels, both in quiet and in noise (adults “soft speech levels”: [Skinner et al, 1999]; [Firszt et al, 2004]; [Dwyer et al, 2016]; children “soft speech levels”: [Davidson, 2006]; [Davidson et al, 2009]; [Baudhuin et al, 2012]; [Robinson et al, 2012]; [Geers et al, 2013]; children +5 dB SNR: [Gifford et al, 2011]; [Sheffield et al, 2015]; children both at high and low levels: [Rakszawski et al, 2016]). Thus, these recommendations are data driven and include stimuli and presentation levels for which feasibility has been documented.

As mentioned previously, this working group aimed to develop a suggested protocol using “currently commercially available” measures rather than to develop new tests. The group selected tests for this battery based on availability, clinical acceptance, ease of administration, availability of normative data for children with normal hearing and/or hearing loss, group consensus, and the ability to transition to a more age- and language-appropriate battery as necessary for each child. Specifically, once the child has reached the ceiling performance levels for tests in this battery, it is expected that the audiologist will transition to those measures outlined in the adult MSTB ([MSTB, 2011]). (It should be noted that one test recommended for the PMSTB, a conditioned head turn task similar to visual reinforcement audiometry called visual reinforcement infant speech discrimination [VRISD], does not have widespread clinical use at this time. However, centers can purchase VRISD commercially to assess infant discrimination. Multiple centers have implemented VRISD as a discrimination metric both in clinic [[Govaerts et al 2006]; [2010]; [Uhler et al, 2011]; [Uhler, 2014]] and in research [[Moore et al, 1975]; [Eilers et al, 1977]; [Nozza, 1987]; [Martinez et al, 2008]; [Uhler et al, 2011]; [Uhler et al, 2015]]. Please see [Supplemental Appendix S1] for further details.)


#

RATIONALE FOR TEST SELECTION

The design of the PMSTB battery matches [Kirk and colleagues’ (2009)] description of a comprehensive battery, which “should permit the evaluation of a hierarchy of skills, ranging from discrimination of vowel and consonant speech features through the comprehension of connected speech” (p. 225). Successful implementation of a test battery depends on the clinician’s ability to understand how to administer the assessment measures and when to administer and/or stop administering particular instruments. This decision must be both easily and quickly executable within a test session.

The PMSTB manual describes the test battery, illustrated as a flowchart in Figure A1 in [Supplemental Appendix S1], and provides information on the administration of particular tests as well as guidelines for transitioning between measures of higher or lower difficulty for a particular child (see Appendix B for ordering details). The following sections describe subtleties associated with selection of a test relative to a child’s ability to respond, language age, articulation abilities, and current auditory skills.

Select Measures That Match the Child’s Ability to Respond

To make the PMSTB relevant for a broad age range including infants, preschoolers, and potentially early school-aged children, the current iteration includes parent questionnaires as well as closed- and open-set measures of speech understanding. Parent questionnaires provide a glimpse into a child’s performance in a real-world environment and supply information for children who cannot complete behavioral measures due to chronological age or developmental level. The parent questionnaires selected for the PMSTB include LittlEARS ([Kuehn-Inacken et al, 2003]; [Coninx et al, 2009]) and the Auditory Skills Checklist ([Meinzen-Derr et al, 2007]). The manual describes the purpose, administration, and scoring of both instruments.

Closed-set tests limit response options to a predetermined, fixed array of items. For example, children can select a response to an auditory stimulus from 1 of 4 tangible items on the Early Speech Perception (ESP) Low Verbal version, 1 of 5 images on the Pediatric Sentence Intelligibility (PSI) test, or 1 of 12 pictures on the ESP Standard Version test ([Jerger et al, 1983]; [Moog and Geers, 1990]). The level of difficulty increases with a greater number of items in the foil. Children with normal hearing often can complete the aforementioned closed-set tests by 3 yr of age ([Robbins and Kirk, 1996]). Children in this age range, who typically exhibit greater receptive versus expressive language, can easily respond via pointing to pictures of objects. The restriction of potential response options in closed-set tests may not necessarily represent real-world listening situations, but it does provide several advantages. First, closed-set tests pose an easier task that young children can complete based on their language and motor abilities. Second, closed-set tests allow children to focus on audition with reductions in the concomitant influence of cognitive-linguistic factors (i.e., expressive and receptive vocabulary, auditory memory) relative to open-set tasks ([Boothroyd 1995]; [Eisenberg et al, 2003]; [2004]). Thus, closed-set tests afford a first glimpse into how a child attaches meaning to sound in a structured manner.

On the other hand, open-set speech perception tests do not limit response possibilities. Children can answer via verbal, gestural, or signed response to word (e.g., “banana, water, please”) or sentence stimuli (e.g., “The baby monkey swings from the trees”). Examples of pediatric open-set speech perception tests include the Multisyllabic Lexical Neighborhood Test ([Kirk, Pisoni, and Osberger, 1995]), Lexical Neighborhood Test (LNT; [Kirk et al, 1995]), the Bamford–Kowal–Bench (BKB) Sentence-in-Noise test ([Etymōtic Research, Inc., 2005]; also see [Bench et al, 1979]), and the Pediatric AzBio test (BabyBio; [Spahr et al, 2014]). Open-set tests have greater real-world application because they do not constrain topics by including pictures or objects to guide attention. Rather, words and sentences that could occur in real conversations may include a wide array of subject areas, organization structure, or key words ([Tyler et al, 1986]; [1987]).


#

Select Measures That Match the Child’s Language Age, Not Chronologic Age

Most tests in the present protocol provide recommended age ranges based on typical development; however, note that the recommended age ranges in the PMSTB may differ slightly from those recommended by the test manuals. These decisions stemmed not only from typical development, but also the breadth and depth of clinical experience represented by the PMSTB working group. Thus, the working group’s recommendations regarding appropriate age ranges should serve as a flexible starting point, remaining mindful of the wide range in language skill levels for children with and without hearing loss.

Children with hearing loss may acquire speech and language skills differently than their peers with normal hearing. As clinical audiologists, we need to exhibit sensitivity to differences in language abilities and not focus solely on chronologic age as a criterion for selection and administration of speech perception tests. This highlighted need for sensitivity comes from the fact that children with hearing loss demonstrate difficulties acquiring not only speech perception skills but also speech production accuracy and receptive and expressive language abilities ([Boothroyd et al, 1991]; [Hayes et al, 2009]; [Tobey et al, 2011]).

Clinicians are better equipped to select an appropriate test when provided with information about the global developmental level and language abilities of a child with hearing loss. For example, an infant who receives a CI at 12 mo of age may not utter his first word until 5–10 mo after CI activation, at a chronologic age of 17–22 mo ([Warner-Czyz and Davis, 2008]). Typically developing hearing peers at the same chronologic age have a much different communication skill set. In infants with typical hearing, first spoken words emerge ∼12 mo of age and the number of new words increases at a slow rate (i.e., 1–3 new words per month) until a “vocabulary spurt,” in which word acquisition increases significantly (i.e., 10–20 new words per week) ∼21 mo of age ([Ganger and Brent, 2004]). This vocabulary spurt reflects the repetition of words over time, variation in word difficulty over time, and the child’s efficiency to learn new words ([Hart and Risley, 1995]; [McMurray, 2007]). Thus, an average 21-mo-old with normal hearing may have a lexicon of nearly 200 words (median = 171 words), whereas a 21-mo-old with a CI may have just 5 spoken words (<5th percentile; [Fenson et al, 2007]). Clinicians can use knowledge of a child’s language level based on parent report and language assessment reports from a speech-language pathologist to select appropriate speech perception tests.

Children using HAs also increase receptive and expressive vocabulary skills over time, but the rate of word acquisition does not always match that of typically developing peers—especially for those with more severe degrees of hearing loss. [Mayne, Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey (2000)] reported that infants and toddlers using HAs understand an average of 14 words between 8 and 10 mo, and 47 words between 14 and 16 mo. These values lag behind the lexicon size of hearing peers, who have median receptive vocabularies of 24–45 words and 126–192 words, respectively—thereby indicating that the receptive vocabulary of children using HAs corresponds more closely to the 5th–10th percentile performance levels at similar ages ([Fenson et al, 2007]). However, a more recent study by [Moeller, Hoover, Putman, Arbataitis, Bohnenkamp, Peterson, Wood, et al (2007)] showed a main effect of age (10–24 mo) but not auditory status on receptive language outcomes in children with HAs.

Differences in lexicon size in toddlers with hearing loss versus those with normal hearing also persist in expressive vocabulary. Median vocabulary size of pediatric HA users increases from 0 to 31 words from 8 to 25 mo ([Mayne, Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Carey, 2000]). These values fall behind median values reported in 24-mo-old, typically developing children (251–344 words), instead matching a lower percentile score (35th) for chronologically younger children (16 mo; [Fenson et al, 2007]). Percentile scores for both receptive and expressive language of toddlers with HAs fall increasingly behind hearing peers, indicating a slower rate of acquisition in children with hearing loss—a phenomena termed “gap opening” ([Moeller, Hoover, Putman, Arbataitis, Bohnenkamp, Peterson, Wood, et al, 2007]; [Yoshinaga-Itano et al, 2010]). The majority of infants are identified and fit with amplification before 6 mo of age, but there continues to be a wide range at age of identification (0.25–60 mo) and fitting of amplification, even in a contemporary group of young children with hearing loss (1.5–72 mo; [Holte et al, 2012]); thus, clinicians also must consider a child’s receptive and expressive vocabulary.

Even more recent studies of pediatric HA users confirm slower language development in children with mild-to-severe hearing loss relative to hearing peers ([Ching et al, 2013]; [Tomblin et al, 2014]; [2015]). For example, [Ching et al (2013)] reported that 3-yr-old children with hearing loss obtained a mean global language score more than one standard deviation poorer than age-matched hearing counterparts. We should acknowledge, however, that children with hearing loss show considerable variability in development of receptive and expressive language skills based on demographic and environmental factors including, but not limited to the following: age at identification of hearing loss, severity of hearing loss, degree of audibility, age at HA fit (<18 mo), chronologic age, social interaction, presence of additional disabilities, and quality of linguistic input ([Yoshinaga-Itano et al, 1998]; [Mayne, Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, 2000]; [Mayne, Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Carey, 2000]; [Fulcher et al, 2012]; [Ching et al, 2013]; [Ambrose et al, 2014]; [Tomblin et al, 2015]).

Recognizing differences in language performance levels as opposed to relying on chronologic age will aid in choosing an appropriate speech perception measure. [Table 1] in [Supplemental Appendix S1] integrates expected receptive and expressive language milestones by chronologic age with appropriate assessment tools for both language and speech perception. The inclusion of typical scores (e.g., 50th percentile) and normative score ranges based on chronologic age will allow clinicians to (a) interpret performance levels as assessed by speech-language pathologists, and (b) compare performance of a child with hearing loss to hearing peers of either the same chronologic age or same listening age. Knowing a child’s language level also will facilitate selection of a suitable speech perception test in which a child can comprehend and participate in the testing process to the best of his or her abilities.


#

Consider Alternative Scoring Methods on Individual Tests

Differences in language abilities influence which test(s) a child can complete. Clinicians should pay attention to not only the child’s language abilities, but also his or her speech production skills. Infants and toddlers with hearing loss often show delays in vocal developmental milestones such as the onset of babbling and first words relative to peers with normal hearing ([Stoel-Gammon and Otomo, 1986]; [Oller and Eilers, 1988]; [Moeller, Hoover, Putman, Arbataitis, Bohnenkamp, Peterson, Lewis, et al, 2007]). Phonetic inventories and production accuracy present another area of difference based on auditory status, but effects differ based on phonetic segment type and auditory technology. Infants and toddlers using HAs expand consonant repertoires more slowly than hearing peers—particularly relative to fricatives and affricates—but show no differences in vowel inventories ([Kent et al, 1987]; [Yoshinaga-Itano and Sedey, 2000]; [Moeller, Hoover, Putman, Arbataitis, Bohnenkamp, Peterson, Lewis, et al, 2007]). Matching speech production to word targets creates greater difficulty such that toddlers with normal hearing outperform those with HAs on consonant accuracy, presence of final consonants in words, and vowel accuracy ([Moeller, Hoover, Putman, Arbataitis, Bohnenkamp, Peterson, Wood, et al, 2007]). For example, it has been reported that children with congenital hearing loss are more likely to omit phonemes that are harder to hear such as /s/ and /z/ ([Stelmachowicz et al, 2002]; [McGuckian and Henry, 2007]; [Koehlinger et al, 2013]).

Children with CIs tend to exhibit greatest production accuracy for the sounds they produce most often (e.g., visible consonants such as /b/ and /m/ and neutral vowels such as /ʌ/ and /ə/) ([Warner-Czyz and Davis, 2008]). This population often experiences articulation difficulties for consonants classified as coronal (e.g., /t/), dorsal (e.g., /k/, /g/), or fricative (e.g., /s/, /ʃ/), and vowels produced in the back of the oral cavity (e.g., /u/, /o/) ([Warner-Czyz and Davis, 2008]; [Warner-Czyz et al, 2010]).

Mispronunciation of these sounds may or may not affect intelligibility by a naïve listener. Tobey and colleagues reported a moderate correlation (r > 0.50) between the percentage of vowels correct and speech intelligibility and a high correlation (r > 0.80) between the percentage of consonants correct and speech intelligibility ([Tobey et al, 2003]; [Tobey et al, 2011]; [2013]). Specifically, stop-plosives (r = 0.59) and fricatives (r = 0.79) strongly correlate with intelligibility by a naïve listener ([Tobey et al, 2003]). Thus, incorrect articulation of stop-plosives (e.g., /t/, /k/, /g/) and fricatives affects speech intelligibility and could, thus, affect clinician ratings of speech perception.

Many of the more advanced speech perception tests use an open-set format, in which verbal responses have an infinite range. Scoring involves calculating a percent correct score at the phoneme, word, key word, or sentence level. However, these scores often build upon each other such that omitting one phoneme or syllable (e.g., /nænə/ for banana) affects not only the phoneme score, but also the word score if strict scoring requires accurate pronunciation of all phonemes to count as correct. The same concept arises for sentence scoring if the child must produce all key words to yield a correct sentence score. Thus, misarticulation—a speech production issue—inadvertently affects scores on multiple speech perception measures.

Clinicians should practice caution in penalizing children for misarticulation on a speech perception test. Appropriate follow-up based on speech perception scores depends on determination if errors relate to an underlying speech perception or speech production (articulation) issue. Device programing by an audiologist ([Tyler et al, 1987]) addresses a speech perception error, whereas therapeutic intervention by a speech-language pathologist is able to aid in determination of a true articulation error versus a developmentally appropriate error. For example, a common perceptual confusion for CI users is /u/ versus /m/ due to the frequency overlap of the first formant and difference in the second formant. However, this is not a common articulation error. Thus, the documentation and subsequent analysis of error patterns may inform both perceptual and production aspects of communication above and beyond a percent correct score.


#

Select Measures That Match the Child’s Current Auditory Skills Level

One of our primary goals as clinicians and researchers focuses on assessing a child’s perceptual skills as accurately as possible based on current auditory skills. The PMSTB provides guidance as to when clinicians should transition to a different test. For example, performance scores greater than 75–80% correct suggest a child has mastered the skills assessed in a particular test and should proceed to the next hierarchical level of difficulty, either in the same testing session or during the next testing session. On the other hand, scores of ∼25% or lower suggest that a simpler task is necessary. The lower limit of this score range is based on chance for a four-choice test being 25% ([Tomblin et al, 1999]). Determining the upper criterion, however, was a more challenging task. The operational definition of a ceiling effect is the maximum possible score for a particular measure. If we were confident that all children with hearing loss could achieve 100% accuracy on each measure of speech perception, then we would have suggested that the clinician progress to the next level of difficulty once a child had achieved a score that was not significantly different from 100% (based on the 95% confidence interval for the chosen measure). Children with hearing loss, however, will likely not achieve a true ceiling effect on all measures, especially speech perception in noise. Thus, the PMSTB working group chose a value in the range of 75–80% to be approaching ceiling, as we expect many children will asymptote at scores <100%. More moderate performance scores (i.e., 25–79% correct) suggest emergence of skills assessed in that measure, thereby suggesting the appropriateness of the measure for continued use in future test sessions. Conversely, once scores reach 80% or higher “on a particular measure,” the clinician should administer the next measure in the hierarchy—either at the same visit (pending child attention and fatigue) or at the next scheduled visit (e.g., transitioning from LNT words to consonant-nucleus-consonant [CNC] words). In this same scenario, regardless of whether the clinician transitions to CNC, the clinician would continue with the hierarchical protocol progressing from words to sentences in quiet and then to sentences in noise. Once a child has achieved mastery (≥80% correct) on the higher level auditory assessments (e.g., BabyBio), future testing can focus on more advanced speech perception tests included in the adult MSTB (e.g., AzBio).

Evaluators expect this forward progression in the acquisition of auditory perceptual skills. However, what happens when a child cannot achieve even 25% correct on a specific task? When a child cannot attain a minimal level of proficiency on a speech perception measure, the PMSTB recommends shifting to an easier perceptual task to meet the child at his or her level of auditory skills. For example, a child scoring <25% correct on the ESP monosyllable task—which presents stimuli differing in vowels only (e.g., “bat, boat, boot”)—should not transition to PSI words, which require monosyllabic differentiation. Rather, that child should revert to the perceptually easier ESP spondee task, which presents two-syllable stimuli with differing consonant and vowel composition (e.g., “hotdog” and “bathtub”).


#

Interpret Outcomes Relative to Other Tests and Previous Performance

Making clinical decisions requires professionals to look beyond test scores on an individual test measure. That is, to comprehensively assess a child’s speech perception abilities, clinicians must consider performance across measures and performance across testing sessions. Clinical decision-making relies not only on absolute scores but also on relative values when comparing performance over time or with different device configurations. A clinician needs to know if a change in performance constitutes a “clinically significant change.” The PMSTB manual provides the 95% confidence interval for test–retest variability on an individual level, by age and for the number of lists where these normative data are available (see Tables 9–11 in [Supplemental Appendix S1]). As mentioned previously, with the implementation of the PMSTB, we anticipate the collection and dissemination of normative data for each measure included in the current and future versions of the PMSTB.

A final fallback to an easier perceptual task is to revert from recorded materials to MLV. The protocol recommends recorded speech perception materials to maintain consistency of speaker intensity, dialect, and intonation, and to avoid the inflation of scores commonly observed with MLV ([Roeser and Clark, 2008]; [Uhler et al, 2016]). Though MLV affords greater flexibility in testing—particularly for very young children and individuals with reduced cognitive function—MLV reduces the reliability of test results, making it impossible to compare across test sessions and testers. Therefore, we recommend that MLV be avoided whenever possible.

Overall, test selection within the PMSTB offers flexibility in terms of starting point as well as forward and backward transition to match the speech perception testing needs of an individual child. Clinicians should pay attention to multiple details such as a child’s ability to respond, language age, articulation, and current auditory skills when evaluating speech perception abilities of a child with hearing loss.


#
#

RATIONALE FOR PROTOCOL DESIGN: MULTIPLE LEVELS AND LISTENING CONFIGURATIONS

Assistive technology such as HAs and CIs can provide children with hearing loss the necessary auditory access to acquire listening and spoken language skills. The benefits of this technology, however, may depend on the stimulus level and listening environment. Thus it is essential that we consider multiple listening scenarios in order to optimize fittings for HA, CI, and bone-anchored implants (BAI). Testing at average conversational speech levels (e.g., 60 dBA) indicates how well a child will understand a talker positioned within a few feet. Testing at lower presentation levels approximating perceptual descriptions of “soft speech” (e.g., 50 dBA) mimics common listening conditions because children rarely have a consistently optimal signal, and perception of low-level speech has potential implications for receptive and expressive language development.

Children with normal hearing commonly acquire language abilities through incidental learning ([Akhtar et al, 2001]) and overall exposure to quality language ([Hart and Risley, 1995]; [Landry et al, 2000]; [Huttenlocher et al, 2002]; [Kashinath et al, 2006]; [Law et al, 2009]; [Suskind et al, 2013]). Thus, we can expect similar if not greater disparities in children with hearing loss, who not only have less exposure to language produced at lower intensity levels but also have compromised stimulus delivery.

Hearing Technology Verification and Validation

Regardless of auditory status, children learn language best not only when they have access to low-level speech, but also when they can access speech at various levels in adverse listening conditions—both of which can be optimized through well-fit devices (e.g., HAs, CIs, BAI, FM/DM) for children with hearing loss. Classrooms, playgrounds, and home environments represent typical listening situations for young children, and all yield an unfavorable signal-to-noise ratio in which children with hearing loss are expected to thrive ([Sanders, 1965]; [Nober and Nober, 1975]; [Bess et al, 1984]; [Finitzo-Hieber, 1988]; [Clark and Govett, 1995]; [Crandell and Smaldino, 1995]; [Crukley et al, 2011]). Thus, it follows that the evaluation of speech recognition in noise should be standard clinical practice for “validation” of HA and/or CI fittings following “verification” of acoustic ear canal SPL for HA, aided warbled-tone thresholds for CI, and verification of BAI output using a combination of audiometric thresholds obtained with direct bone conduction or measurement of processor output via coupling to a skull simulator. In summary, the stimulus presentation levels included in the PMSTB were chosen on the basis of (a) ecological validity as these represent average levels of speech and noise most frequently encountered in everyday listening environments for both pediatric and adult listeners ([Pearsons et al, 1977]; [Clark and Govett, 1995]; [Olsen, 1998]; [Crukley et al, 2011]; [Smeds et al, 2015]), and (b) feasibility documented in the peer-reviewed literature for presentation at levels ranging from 50 to 60 dBA in quiet and higher in the presence of noise ([Firszt et al, 2004]; [Davidson, 2006]; [Davidson et al, 2009]; [Gifford et al, 2011]; [Baudhuin et al, 2012]; [Robinson et al, 2012]; [Geers et al, 2013]; [Sheffield et al, 2015]; [Dwyer et al, 2016]; [Rakszawski et al, 2016]; ). Similarly, the PMSTB working group’s primary concern centered on defining stimulus parameters that would gauge how well a child was performing for stimulus and noise levels typically encountered, rather than designing a protocol that would simply yield high outcomes. If children with hearing loss exhibit significant difficulty at SNRs most commonly encountered in typical listening environments for preschool- and school-aged children—such as +5 dB SNR—then this provides clinicians with diagnostically relevant information that can guide clinical decision-making. For example, this information could guide clinical recommendations for additional intervention such as initial cochlear implantation, pursuing a second implant, programming different acoustic gain and/or HA characteristics, using CART services in the classroom, full-time use of FM/DM technology, etc.

Normal development of auditory skills depends upon audibility for low-, mid-, and high-level sounds, including speech. The amount of amplification applied to low-level sounds must not interfere with the need to maintain a usable temporal envelope (e.g., preserve speech peaks) and to avoid excessive amplification of noisy signals. Monitoring a child’s speech perception requires measures appropriate for a child’s chronologic age, cognitive status, language abilities and audibility at multiple intensities (i.e., normal and soft conversational levels) and in multiple listening environments (i.e., in quiet and in competing noise). The PMSTB addresses all points with a standardized protocol appropriate for children with a range of abilities from discrimination in quiet to sentence recognition in noise, and in a variety of settings, from clinic to research.


#

Limitations

Though the PMSTB offers great benefits to professional and patients, as a working group, we would like to acknowledge that this first iteration has its limitations. First, a limited amount of normative data exists for the current PMSTB measures. This restricts our ability to benchmark a child’s static performance and progress over time against typically developing, hearing peers. Second, some of the validated measures selected for the protocol have a limited number of equivalent lists. For some of the PMSTB measures, this constraint prohibits independent assessment of all listening conditions (e.g., left ear, right ear, and bilateral) within a single session. Newer measures have emerged since the initial development of this recommended test battery and could, at some point, become part of the recommended protocol. We have always anticipated that the PMSTB would evolve over time with increased knowledge about development and skills in this population.

As mentioned previously, one of the primary goals for creating a standardized protocol is that, over time, it may afford the development of age-normative data and test–retest variability estimates. This will, in turn, allow reliable benchmarking of patient performance and determination of clinically significant changes based on binomial distribution statistics. Furthermore, we both anticipate and encourage test development including an adequately large number of lists for the accurate and independent evaluation of speech and word recognition in various listening configuration as well as longitudinal assessment.


#
#

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS

Measurement of patient outcomes and documentation of treatment efficacy represents an essential component of (re)habilitative audiology. While one could argue that outcome measures themselves do not improve patient outcomes, the adoption and adherence to a standardized assessment protocol can facilitate continuity of care, assist in clinical decision-making, and allow benchmarking against both hearing peers as well as our aggregate clinical population. Additionally, a uniform test battery could aid patient and family counseling regarding expectations and predictions for improvement over time. We expect that the PSMTB will transform over time and as new tests, upgraded technologies, and knowledge about this young population from larger patient populations become available. In the meantime, however, professionals serving families with children with hearing loss cannot allow current limitations impede the development and implementation of standardized assessment battery.

Although the working group wholeheartedly supports establishment of a standardized assessment protocol, we want to emphasize that the PMSTB represents a “minimum” test battery for use with all children at every visit. Individual clinics and clinicians can administer additional assessments at their discretion based on professional judgment and the child’s needs.

Review of the current literature highlights a lack of consistency in accepted assessment protocols across laboratories, clinics, and even among clinicians within the same clinic (e.g., [Uhler and Gifford, 2014]). Given the changing nature of our national healthcare system and federal initiatives designed at improving the quality and efficiency of healthcare and service delivery—including a pay-for-performance model of reimbursement—we can expect that the adoption and implementation of a standardized assessment battery for children with hearing loss will, at a minimum, become the norm. Thus, this PMSTB working group of clinicians, scientists, and industry representatives has developed the first iteration of the PMSTB, which is included in [Supplemental Appendix S1]. Implementation of the PMSTB in our clinical practice and dissemination of associated data are both critical for achieving the next level of success for our patients and for elevating pediatric audiology, (re)habilitative audiology, as well as pediatric CI and HA research.


#

APPENDIX A

Zoom Image

#

APPENDIX B

Details for Ordering Specific Test Measures

Test

Authors (Year)

Ordering/Download Information

Auditory Skills Checklist (ASC)

Meinzen-Derr et al (2004)

Annals of Otology, Rhinology, and Laryngology, 116(11):812–818.

Bamford–KowalBench (BKB) Sentences in Quiet and in Noise (BKB-SIN)

[Bench et al (1979)]; [Etymotic Research (2005)]

Auditec (www.auditec.com) Etymotic Research (www.etymotic.com)

Consonant-Nucleus-Consonant (CNC)

[Peterson and Lehiste (1962)]

Bio-logic Systems Corp. (http://www.bionicear.com/For_Professionals/Audiology_Support/CNC_Test.cfm?)

Early Speech Perception Test

[Moog and Geers (1990)]

Central Institute for the Deaf (http://www.cid.edu/ProfOutreachIntro/EducationalMaterials.aspx)

Lexical Neighborhood Test (LNT)

[Kirk et al (1995)]

Auditec (www.auditec.com)

LittlEars Auditory Questionnaire

Kuhn-Inacker et al (2003)

Med El (http://s3.medel.com/downloadmanager/downloads/bridge_us/en-US/BRIDGE_Order_Form.pdf)

Multisyllabic Lexical Neighborhood Test (MLNT)

[Kirk et al (1995)]

Auditec (www.auditec.com)

Pediatric AzBio Sentence Lists

[Spahr et al (2014)]

Auditory Potential (http://www.auditorypotential.com/purchase.html)

Pediatric Speech Intelligibility (PSI)

Jerger and Jerger (1984)

Auditec (www.auditec.com)


#

Abbreviations

BAI: bone-anchored implants
BKB: Bamford–Kowal–Bench
CDaCI: Childhood Development after Cochlear Implantation
CI: cochlear implant
CNC: consonant-nucleus-consonant
ESP: Early Speech Perception
FM: frequency modulation
HA: hearing aid
LNT: Lexical Neighborhood Test
LOCHI: Longitudinal Outcomes of Children with Hearing Impairment
MLV: monitored live voice
OCHL: Outcomes of Children with Hearing Loss study
PMSTB: Pediatric Minimum Speech Test Battery
PSI: Pediatric Sentence Intelligibility
VRISD: visual reinforcement infant speech discrimination


#

No conflict of interest has been declared by the author(s).

Acknowledgments

PMSTB Working Group. The Pediatric Minimum Speech Test Battery (PMSTB) working group consisted of 56 clinicians and researchers from across the United States. Kristin Uhler served as the chair of the working group. René Gifford and Andrea Warner-Czyz acted as co-chairs of the working group.

Full authorship list: Kristin Uhler, PhD, University of Colorado Denver School of Medicine, Aurora, CO; René H. Gifford, PhD, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN; Andrea Warner-Czyz, PhD, The University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX; Mary Archer, AuD, West Virginia University HealthCare Children's Hospital; Katie Austin, AuD, Kentucky Hearing Clinic, Louisville, KY; Mary M. “Peg“ Barry, AuD, Commission for Children with Special Health Care Needs, Louisville, KY; Allison Biever, AuD, Rocky Mountain ENT, Englewood, CO; Michelle Blanchard, AuD, Tampa Bay Hearing and Balance, Tampa, FL; Rebecca Brashears, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC; Claire Buxton, AuD, University of Maryland, College Park, MD; Tina Childress, AuD, CASE Audiology, Champaign, IL; Rachel Cooper, AuD, University of Mississippi Medical Center, Jackson, MS; Beth Czarnecki, AuD, Penn State Hershey, Hershey, PA; Laurie Eisenberg, PhD, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA; Shannon Elam, AuD, University of Colorado Hearing and Balance Center, Aurora, CO; Melissa Ferrello, AuD, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia, PA; Lia Ferro, AuD, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL; Hillary Gazeley, AuD, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI; Ann Geers, PhD, The University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX; Katelyn Glassman, AuD, MED-EL Corporation, Raleigh Durham, NC; Janet Green, AuD, NYU Langone Medical Center, New York, NY; Aimee Gross, MS, MED-EL Corporation, Raleigh Durham, NC; Carmen Hayman, AuD, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia, PA; Nikki Herrod-Burrows, AuD, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC; Kathi Henion, AuD, Advanced Bionics, Sylmar, CA; Michelle Hughes, PhD, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, NE; Yell Inverso, AuD, PhD, Nemours/Alfred I. duPont Hospital for Children, Wilmington, DE; Karen Johnson, PhD, Keck School of Medicine USC, Los Angeles, CA; Karen Kirk, PhD, The University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana-Champaign, IL; Kirsten Kramer, AuD, Helen DeVos Children's Hospital, Grand Rapids, MI; Leslie Lianos, MS, Dallas Ear Institute, Dallas, TX; Liesl Looney, AuD, Nemours/Alfred I. duPont Hospital for Children, Wilmington, DE; Jane Madell, PhD, Pediatric Audiology Consulting, Brooklyn, NY; Deanna Matusik, AuD, University of Illinois Medical Center, Chicago, IL; Shelley Moats, AuD, Little Ears Hearing Center, Louisville, KY; Lori O'Neill, AuD, Cochlear Americas, Centennial, CO; Kathryn Pegan, AuD, Clinical Trainer at Phonak, LLC, Warrenville, IL; Pritesh Pandya, PhD, MED-EL Corporation, Raleigh Durham, NC; Sarah F. Poissant, PhD, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA; Wendy Potts, AuD, Cochlear Corporation, Denver, CO; Allison Ramakrishnan, AuD, University of Colorado Hearing and Balance Center, Aurora, CO; Jennifer Raulie, MA, Advanced Bionics, Sylmar, CA; Riza Razack, AuD, Director of Riza Razack Consulting, North York, ON, Canada; Michael Scott, AuD, Cincinnati Children's, Cincinnati, OH; Jared Shifflet, M.A. Advanced Bionics, Sylmar, CA; Carrie Spangler, AuD, Summit County Educational Service Center, Cleveland, OH; Melissa Sweeney, MS, The University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX; Sarah Sydlowski, AuD, PhD, Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH; Holly Teagle, AuD, University of North Carolina School of Medicine, Chapel Hill, NC; Denise Thomas, AuD, Ann and Robert H. Lurie Children's Hospital of Chicago, Chicago, IL; Nanette Thompson, MS, Listen Foundation, Englewood, CO; Julie Verhoff, AuD, The River School, Washington, DC; Dawn Violetto, AuD, Child's Voice, Wood Dale, IL; Rachel Vovos, AuD, Advanced Bionics, Sylmar, CA; Cynthia Warner, AuD, Nationwide Children's Hospital, Columbus, OH; Julia Webb, AuD, Arkansas Children's Hospital, Little Rock, AR; Louisa Yong Yan Ha, AuD, Thomas Jefferson University Hospitals, Philadelphia, PA; Christie Yoshinaga-Itano, PhD, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO; Terry Zwolan, PhD, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI.

Supplementary Material

  • REFERENCES

  • Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) 2011 Technology assessment: effectiveness of cochlear implants in adults with sensorineural hearing loss. Contract no. 290 2007 10055 1). https://www.cms.gov/Medicare/Coverage/DeterminationProcess/downloads/id80TA.pdf Accessed August 1, 2016
  • Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) 2014 Methods guide for comparative effectiveness comparative reviews. Publication no. 10(14)-EHC063-EF. https://effectivehealthcare.ahrq.gov/ehc/products/60/318/CER-Methods-Guide-140109.pdf Accessed August 1, 2016
  • Akhtar N, Jipson J, Callanan MA. 2001; Learning words through overhearing. Child Dev 72 (02) 416-430
  • Ambrose SE, Berry LMU, Walker EA, Harrison M, Oleson J, Moeller MP. 2014; Speech sound production in 2-year-olds who are hard of hearing. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 23: 91-104
  • Baudhuin J, Cadieux J, Firszt JB, Reeder RM, Maxson JL. 2012; Optimization of programming parameters in children with the advanced bionics cochlear implant. J Am Acad Audiol 23 (05) 302-312
  • Bench J, Kowal A, Bamford J. 1979; The BKB (Bamford-Kowal-Bench) sentence lists for partially-hearing children. Br J Audiol 13 (03) 108-112
  • Bess FH, Sinclair JS, Riggs DE. 1984; Group amplification in schools for the hearing impaired. Ear Hear 5 (03) 138-144
  • Beyea JA, McMullen KP, Harris MS, Houston DM, Martin JM., Bolster VA, Adunka OF, Moberly AC. 2016; Cochlear implants in adults: effects of age and duration of deafness on speech recognition. Otol Neurotol 37 (09) 1238-1245
  • Boothroyd A. 1995. Speech perception tests and hearing impaired children. In: Plant G, Spens KE. Profound Deafness and Speech Communication. London, UK: Whurr;
  • Boothroyd A, Geers AE, Moog JS. 1991; Practical implications of cochlear implants in children. Ear Hear 12 (04) (Suppl) 81S-89S
  • Brand Y, Senn P, Kompis M, Dillier N, Allum JH. 2014; Cochlear implantation in children and adults in Switzerland. Swiss Med Wkly 144: w13909
  • Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services 2004 The CMS approved coverage with evidence development (CED) study protocol CAG-00107N, CMS. . https://www.cms.gov/medicare-coverage-database/details/nca-decision-memo.aspx?NCAId=134&NcaName=Cochlear+Implantation&NCDId=245&ncdver=2&IsPopup=y&bc=AAAAAAAAIAAA& December 21, 2016.
  • Ching TYC, Leigh G, Dillon H. 2013; Introduction to the longitudinal outcomes of children with hearing impairment (LOCHI) study: background, design, sample characteristics. Int J Audiol 52 (2, Suppl) S4-S9
  • Clark W, Govett S. 1995. School-related noise exposure in children . Paper presented at the Association for Research in in Otolaryngology Mid-Winter Meeting; St. Petersburg, FL:
  • Coninx F, Weichbold V, Tsiakpini L. et al. 2009; Validation of the LittlEARS((R)) Auditory Questionnaire in children with normal hearing. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 73 (12) 1761-1768
  • Crandell C, Smaldino J. 1995; An update of classroom acoustics for children with hearing impairment. Volta Review 1: 4-12
  • Crukley J, Scollie S, Parsa V. 2011; An exploration of non-quiet listening at school. J Ed Audiol 17: 23-35
  • Davidson LS. 2006; Effects of stimulus level on the speech perception abilities of children using cochlear implants or digital hearing aids. Am J Audiol 15 (02) 141-153
  • Davidson LS, Skinner MW, Holstad BA, Fears BT, Richter MK, Matusofsky M, Brenner C, Holden T, Birath A, Kettel JL, Scollie S. 2009; The effect of instantaneous input dynamic range setting on the speech perception of children with the nucleus 24 implant. Ear Hear 30 (03) 340-349
  • Desai S, Stickney G, Zeng FG. 2008; Auditory-visual speech perception in normal-hearing and cochlear-implant listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 123 (01) 428-440
  • Dorman MF, Cook S, Spahr A, Zhang T, Loiselle L, Schramm D, Whittingham J, Gifford R.. 2015; Factors constraining the benefit to speech understanding of combining information from low-frequency hearing and a cochlear implant. Hear Res 322: 107-111
  • Dorman M, Spahr A, Gifford RH, Cook S, Zhang T, Loiselle L, Whittingham J, Schramm D.. 2012; Bilateral and bimodal benefit as a function of age for adults. J Hear Sci 2 (04) 37-39
  • Dwyer RT, Spahr T, Agrawal S, Hetlinger C, Holder JT, Gifford RH. 2016; Participant-generated cochlear implant programs: speech recognition, sound quality, and satisfaction. Otol Neurotol 37 (07) e209-e216
  • Eilers RE, Wilson WR, Moore JM. 1977; Developmental changes in speech discrimination in infants. J Acoust Soc Am 20: 766-780
  • Eisenberg LS, Johnson KC, Martinez AS, Cokely CG, Tobey EA, Quittner AL, Fink NE, Wang NY, Niparko JK. CDaCI Investigative Team (2006) Speech recognition at 1-year follow-up in the childhood development after cochlear implantation study: methods and preliminary findings. Audiol Neurootol 11 (04) 259-268
  • Eisenberg LS, Martinez AS, Boothroyd A. 2003; Auditory-visual and auditory-only perception of phonetic contrasts in children. Volta Review 103: 327-346
  • Eisenberg LS, Boothroyd A, Martinez AS. 2004. Cochlear Implants International Congress Series. The perception of phonetic contrasts in infants: development of the VRASPAC. Vol. 1273, pp. 364-367 Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier;
  • Eshraghi AA, Nazarian R, Telischi FF, Martinez D, Hodges A, Velandia S, Cejas-Cruz I, Balkany TJ, Lo K, Lang D. 2015; Cochlear implantation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Otol Neurotol 36 (08) e121-e128
  • Etymōtic Research, Inc 2005 BKB-SIN test. Speech-in-Noise Test Version 1.03, 2005. 61 Martin Lane, Elk Grove Village, IL. www.etymotic.com/auditory-research/speech-in-noise-tests/bkb-sin.html Accessed December 21, 2016.
  • Eze N, Ofo E, Jiang D, O’Connor AF. 2013; Systematic review of cochlear implantation in children with developmental disability. Otol Neurotol 34 (08) 1385-1393
  • Fenson L, Marchman VA, Thal DJ, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E. 2007. MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual. 2nd ed. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing;
  • Finitzo-Hieber T (1988) Classroom acoustics. In: Roeser R. Auditory Disorders in School Children. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Thieme-Stratton; 221-223
  • Fink NE, Wang NY, Visaya J, Niparko JK, Quittner A, Eisenberg LS, Tobey EA. CDACI Investigative Team (2007) Childhood Development after Cochlear Implantation (CDaCI) study: design and baseline characteristics. Cochlear Implants Int 8 (02) 92-116
  • Firszt JB, Holden LK, Skinner MW, Tobey EA, Peterson A, Gaggl W, Runge-Samuelson CL, Wackym PA. 2004; Recognition of speech presented at soft to loud levels by adult cochlear implant recipients of three cochlear implant systems. Ear Hear 25 (04) 375-387
  • Fulcher A, Purcell AA, Baker E, Munro N. 2012; Listen up: children with early identified hearing loss achieve age-appropriate speech/language outcomes by 3 years-of-age. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 76 (12) 1785-1794
  • Ganger J, Brent MR. 2004; Reexamining the vocabulary spurt. Dev Psychol 40 (04) 621-632
  • Geers AE, Davidson LS, Uchanski RM, Nicholas JG. 2013; Interdependence of linguistic and indexical speech perception skills in school-age children with early cochlear implantation. Ear Hear 34 (05) 562-574
  • Gifford RH, Dorman MF, Sheffield SW, Teece K, Olund AP. 2014; Availability of binaural cues for bilateral implant recipients and bimodal listeners with and without preserved hearing in the implanted ear. Audiol Neurootol 19 (01) 57-71
  • Gifford RH, Olund AP, Dejong M. 2011; Improving speech perception in noise for children with cochlear implants. J Am Acad Audiol 22 (09) 623-632
  • Gifford RH, Shallop JK, Peterson AM. 2008; Speech recognition materials and ceiling effects: considerations for cochlear implant programs. Audiol Neurootol 13 (03) 193-205
  • Govaerts PJ, Daemers K, Yperman M, De Beukelaer C, De Saegher G, De Ceulaer G. 2006; Auditory speech sounds evaluation (A(section)E): a new test to assess detection, discrimination and identification in hearing impairment. Cochlear Implants Int 7 (02) 92-106
  • Govaerts PJ, Vaerenberg B, De Ceulaer G, Daemers K, De Beukelaer C, Schauwers K. 2010; Development of a software tool using deterministic logic for the optimization of cochlear implant processor programming. Otol Neurotol 31 (06) 908-918
  • Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. 2009; Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform 42 (02) 377-381
  • Hart TR, Risley B. 1995. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing;
  • Hayes H, Geers AE, Treiman R, Moog JS. 2009; Receptive vocabulary development in deaf children with cochlear implants: achievement in an intensive auditory-oral educational setting. Ear Hear 30 (01) 128-135
  • Hillock-Dunn A, Buss E, Duncan N, Roush PA, Leibold LJ. 2014; Effects of nonlinear frequency compression on speech identification in children with hearing loss. Ear Hear 35 (03) 353-365
  • Hillock-Dunn A, Taylor C, Buss E, Leibold LJ. 2015; Assessing speech perception in children with hearing loss: what conventional clinical tools may miss. Ear Hear 36 (02) e57-e60
  • Holte L, Walker E, Oleson J, Spratford M, Moeller MP, Roush P, Ou H, Tomblin JB. 2012; Factors influencing follow-up to newborn hearing screening for infants who are hard of hearing. Am J Audiol 21 (02) 163-174
  • Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Cymerman E, Levine S. 2002; Language input and child syntax. Cognit Psychol 45 (03) 337-374
  • Inscoe JR, Bones C. 2016; Additional difficulties associated with aetiologies of deafness: outcomes from a parent questionnaire of 540 children using cochlear implants. Cochlear Implants Int 17 (01) 21-30
  • Jerger S, Jerger J, Abrams S. 1983; Speech audiometry in the young child. Ear Hear 4 (01) 56-66
  • Kashinath S, Woods J, Goldstein H. 2006; Enhancing generalized teaching strategy use in daily routines by parents of children with autism. J Speech Lang Hear Res 49 (03) 466-485
  • Kent RD, Osberger MJ, Netsell R, Hustedde CG. 1987; Phonetic development in identical twins differing in auditory function. J Speech Hear Disord 52 (01) 64-75
  • Kirk KI, French B, Choi S. 2009. Assessing spoken word recognition in children with cochlear implants. In: Eisenberg LS. Clinical Management of Children with Cochlear Implants. San Diego, CA: Plural Publishers;
  • Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Osberger MJ. 1995; Lexical effects on spoken word recognition by pediatric cochlear implant users. Ear Hear 16 (05) 470-481
  • Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Sommers MS, Young M, Evanson C. 1995; New directions for assessing speech perception in persons with sensory aids. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl 166: 300-303
  • Koch DB, Quick A, Osberger MJ, Saoji A, Litvak L. 2014; Enhanced hearing in noise for cochlear implant recipients: clinical trial results for a commercially available speech-enhancement strategy. Otol Neurotol 35 (05) 803-809
  • Koehlinger KM, Van Horne AJO, Moeller MP. 2013; Grammatical outcomes of 3- and 6-year-old children who are hard of hearing. J Speech Lang Hear Res 56 (05) 1701-1714
  • Kuehn-Inacken H, Weichboldt V, Tsiakpini L, Coninx F, D’Haese P. (2003) LittlEARS Auditory questionnaire: parents questionnaire to assess auditory behaviour. Innsbruck, Austria: MedEl.
  • Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR, Miller-Loncar CL. 2000; Early maternal and child influences on children’s later independent cognitive and social functioning. Child Dev 71 (02) 358-375
  • Law J, Rush R, Schoon I, Parsons S. 2009; Modeling developmental language difficulties from school entry into adulthood: literacy, mental health, and employment outcomes. J Speech Lang Hear Res 52 (06) 1401-1416
  • Leibold LJ, Hillock-Dunn A, Duncan N, Roush PA, Buss E. 2013; Influence of hearing loss on children’s identification of spondee words in a speech-shaped noise or a two-talker masker. Ear Hear 34 (05) 575-584
  • Olsen WO. 1998; Average speech levels and spectra in various speaking/listening conditions: a summary of the Pearson, Bennett, and Fidell (1977) report. Am J Audiol 7 (02) 21-25
  • Mahmoud AF, Ruckenstein MJ. 2014; Speech perception performance as a function of age at implantation among postlingually deaf adult cochlear implant recipients. Otol Neurotol 35 (10) e286-e291
  • Martinez A, Eisenberg L, Boothroyd A, Visser-Dumont L. 2008; Assessing speech pattern contrast perception in infants: Early results on VRASPAC. Otol Neuroto 29: 183-188
  • Massa ST, Ruckenstein MJ. 2014; Comparing the performance plateau in adult cochlear implant patients using HINT and AzBio. Otol Neurotol 35 (04) 598-604
  • Mayne A, Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey AL. 2000; Receptive vocabulary development of infants and toddlers who are deaf or hard of hearing. Volta Review 100: 29-52
  • Mayne AM, Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey AL, Carey A. 2000; Expressive vocabulary development of infants and toddlers who are deaf or hard of hearing. Volta Review 100: 1-28
  • McCreery RW, Alexander J, Brennan MA, Hoover B, Kopun J, Stelmachowicz PG. 2014; The influence of audibility on speech recognition with nonlinear frequency compression for children and adults with hearing loss. Ear Hear 35 (04) 440-447
  • McCreery RW, Walker EA, Spratford M, Oleson J, Bentler R, Holte L, Roush P. 2015; Speech recognition and parent ratings From auditory development questionnaires in children who are hard of hearing. Ear Hear 36 (1, Suppl) 60S-75S
  • McGuckian M, Henry A. 2007; The grammatical morpheme deficit in moderate hearing impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord 42 (1, Suppl) 17-36
  • McMurray B. 2007; Defusing the childhood vocabulary explosion. Science 317 (5838) 631
  • Meinzen-Derr J, Wiley S, Creighton J, Choo D. 2007; Auditory Skills Checklist: clinical tool for monitoring functional auditory skill development in young children with cochlear implants. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 116 (11) 812-818
  • Minimum-Speech-Test-Battery (MSTB) 2011 The new minimum speech test battery. . http://www.auditorypotential.com/MSTB_Nav.html Accessed December 21, 2016
  • Moeller MP, Hoover B, Putman C, Arbataitis K, Bohnenkamp G, Peterson B, Lewis D, Estee S, Pittman A, Stelmachowicz P. 2007; Vocalizations of infants with hearing loss compared with infants with normal hearing: part I—phonetic development. Ear Hear 28 (05) 605-627
  • Moeller MP, Hoover B, Putman C, Arbataitis K, Bohnenkamp G, Peterson B, Wood S, Lewis D, Pittman A, Stelmachowicz P. 2007; Vocalizations of infants with hearing loss compared with infants with normal hearing: part II—transition to words. Ear Hear 28 (05) 628-642
  • Moog J, Geers A. 1990. Early Speech Perception Test. St. Louis, MO: Central Institute for the Deaf;
  • Moore J, Thompson G, Thompson M. 1975; Auditory localization of infants as a function of reinforcement conditions. J Speech Lang Hear Dis 40: 29-34
  • Niparko JK, Tobey EA, Thal DJ, Eisenberg LS, Wang NY, Quittner AL, Fink NE. CDaCI Investigative Team (2010) Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. JAMA 303 (15) 498-506
  • Nober L, Nober E. 1975; Auditory discrimination of learning disabled children in quiet and classroom noise. J Learn Disabil 8: 656-659
  • Nozza RJ. 1987; Infant speech-sound discrimination testing: Effects of stimulus intensity and procedural model on measures of performance. J Acoust Soc Am 81: 1928-1939
  • Olds C, Pollonini L, Abaya H, Larky J, Loy M, Bortfeld H, Beauchamp MS, Oghalai JS. 2016; Cortical activation patterns correlate with speech understanding after cochlear implantation. Ear Hear 37 (03) e160-e172
  • Oller DK, Eilers RE. 1988; The role of audition in infant babbling. Child Dev 59 (02) 441-449
  • Osberger MJ, Miyamoto RT, Zimmerman-Phillips S, Kemink JL, Stroer BS, Firszt JB, Novak MA. 1991; Independent evaluation of the speech perception abilities of children with the Nucleus 22-channel cochlear implant system. Ear Hear 12 (04) (Suppl) 66S-80S
  • Pearsons K, Bennett R, Fidell S. 1977. Speech Levels in Various Environments. Washington, DC: Office of Health and Ecological Effects, Office of Research and Development, US EPA:
  • Pediatric Minimum Speech Test Battery Working Group (PMSTB) 2013 Meeting minutes, project updates and file cabinets. . https://sites.google.com/site/pediatricmstbworkinggroup/home Accessed December 21, 2016
  • Peterson GE, Lehiste I. 1962; Revised CNC lists for auditory tests. J Speech Hear Disord 27: 62-70
  • Rakszawski B, Wright R, Cadieux JH, Davidson LS, Brenner C. 2016; The effects of preprocessing strategies for pediatric cochlear implant recipients. J Am Acad Audiol 27 (02) 85-102
  • Robbins AM, Kirk KI. 1996; Speech perception assessment and performance in pediatric cochlear implant users. Semin Hear 17: 353-369
  • Robinson EJ, Davidson LS, Uchanski RM, Brenner CM, Geers AE. 2012; A longitudinal study of speech perception skills and device characteristics of adolescent cochlear implant users. J Am Acad Audiol 23 (05) 341-349
  • Roeser R, Clark J. 2008; Live voice speech recognition audiometry: stop the madness. Audiol Today 20: 32-33
  • Roland Jr JT, Gantz BJ, Waltzman SB, Parkinson AJ. Multicenter Clinical Trial Group (2016) United States multicenter clinical trial of the cochlear nucleus hybrid implant system. Laryngoscope 126 (01) 175-181
  • Runge CL, Henion K, Tarima S, Beiter A, Zwolan TA. 2016; Clinical outcomes of the Cochlear™ Nucleus(®) 5 Cochlear Implant System and SmartSound™ 2 Signal Processing. J Am Acad Audiol 27 (06) 425-440
  • Sanders DA. 1965; Noise conditions in normal school classrooms. Except Child 31: 344-353
  • Sarant JZ, Holt CM, Dowell RC, Rickards FW, Blamey PJ. 2009; Spoken language development in oral preschool children with permanent childhood deafness. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 14 (02) 205-217
  • Schafer EC. 2010; Speech perception in noise measures for children: a critical review and case studies. J. Ed Audiol 16: 4-15
  • Sheffield SW, Haynes DS, Wanna GB, Labadie RF, Gifford RH. 2015; Availability of binaural cues for pediatric bilateral cochlear implant recipients. J Am Acad Audiol 26 (03) 289-298
  • Skinner MW, Holden LK, Holden TA, Demorest ME. 1999; Comparison of two methods for selecting minimum stimulation levels used in programming the Nucleus 22 cochlear implant. J Speech Lang Hear Res 42 (04) 814-828
  • Smeds K, Wolters F, Rung M. 2015; Estimation of signal-to-noise ratio in realistic sound scenarios. J Am Acad Audiol 26 (02) 183-196
  • Spahr AJ, Dorman MF, Litvak LM, Cook SJ, Loiselle LM, DeJong MD, Hedley-Williams A, Sunderhaus LS, Hayes CA, Gifford RH. 2014; Development and validation of the pediatric AzBio sentence lists. Ear Hear 35 (04) 418-422
  • Spahr AJ, Dorman MF, Loiselle LH. 2007; Performance of patients using different cochlear implant systems: effects of input dynamic range. Ear Hear 28 (02) 260-275
  • Stelmachowicz P, Lewis D, Hoover B, Nishi K, McCreery R, Woods W. 2010; Effects of digital noise reduction on speech perception for children with hearing loss. Ear Hear 31 (03) 345-355
  • Stelmachowicz PG, Pittman AL, Hoover BM, Lewis DE. 2002; Aided perception of /s/ and /z/ by hearing-impaired children. Ear Hear 23 (04) 316-324
  • Stoel-Gammon C, Otomo K. 1986; Babbling development of hearing-impaired and normally hearing subjects. J Speech Hear Disord 51 (01) 33-41
  • Suskind D, Leffel KR, Hernandez MW, Sapolich SG, Suskind E, Kirkham E, Meehan P. 2013; An exploratory study of “quantitative linguistic feedback”: effect of LENA feedback on adult language production. Comm Disord Q 34 (04) 199-209
  • Tobey EA, Geers AE, Brenner C, Altuna D, Gabbert G. 2003; Factors associated with development of speech production skills in children implanted by age five. Ear Hear 24 (01) (Suppl) 36S-45S
  • Tobey EA, Geers AE, Sundarrajan M, Shin S. 2011; Factors influencing speech production in elementary and high school-aged cochlear implant users. Ear Hear 32 (01) (Suppl) 27S-38S
  • Tobey EA, Thal D, Niparko JK, Eisenberg LS, Quittner AL, Wang NY. CDaCI Investigative Team (2013) Influence of implantation age on school-age language performance in pediatric cochlear implant users. Int J Audiol 52 (04) 219-229
  • Tomblin JB, Oleson JJ, Ambrose SE, Walker E, Moeller MP. 2014; The influence of hearing aids on the speech and language development of children with hearing loss. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 140 (05) 403-409
  • Tomblin JB, Spencer L, Flock S, Tyler R, Gantz B. 1999; A comparison of language achievement in children with cochlear implants and children using hearing aids. J Speech Lang Hear Res 42 (02) 497-509
  • Tomblin JB, Walker EA, McCreery RW, Arenas RM, Harrison M, Moeller MP. 2015; Outcomes of children with hearing loss: data collection and methods. Ear Hear 36 (1, Suppl 1) 14S-23S
  • Tyler RS, Berliner KI, Demorest ME, Hirshorn MS, Luxford WM, Mangham CA. 1986; Clinical objectives and research design issues for cochlear implants in children. Semin Hear 7: 433-440
  • Tyler RS, Davis JM, Lansing CR. 1987; Cochlear implants in young children. ASHA 29 (04) 41-49
  • Uhler K. 2014 Relationship between speech perception and language. Ultimate Colorado Midwinter Meeting, Vail, CO, February 2014
  • Uhler KM, Baca R, Dudas E, Fredrickson T. 2015; Refining stimulus parameters in assessing infant speech perception using visual reinforcement infant speech discrimination: sensation level. J Am Acad Audiol 26 (10) 807-814
  • Uhler K, Biever A, Gifford RH. 2016; Method of speech stimulus presentation impacts pediatric speech recognition: monitored live voice versus recorded speech. Otol Neurotol 37 (02) e70-e74
  • Uhler K, Burns S, Dalpes M, Yoshinaga-Itano C (2011) An auditory spoken language matrix for differential diagnosis of Spanish-speaking children who are deaf or hard of hearing. ASHA Spec Interes Group 1418:71–78. doi: 10.1044/cds18.3.71
  • Uhler K, Gifford RH. 2014; Current trends in pediatric cochlear implant candidate selection and postoperative follow-up. Am J Audiol 23 (03) 309-325
  • Waltzman S, Cohen NL, Spivak L, Ying E, Brackett D, Shapiro W, Hoffman R. 1990; Improvement in speech perception and production abilities in children using a multichannel cochlear implant. Laryngoscope 100 (03) 240-243
  • Wang NY, Eisenberg LS, Johnson KC, Fink NE, Tobey EA, Quittner AL, Niparko JK. CDaCI Investigative Team (2008) Tracking development of speech recognition: longitudinal data from hierarchical assessments in the Childhood Development after Cochlear Implantation Study. Otol Neurotol 29 (02) 240-245
  • Warner-Czyz AD, Davis BL. 2008; The emergence of segmental accuracy in young cochlear implant recipients. Cochlear Implants Int 9 (03) 143-166
  • Warner-Czyz AD, Davis BL, MacNeilage PF. 2010; Accuracy of consonant-vowel syllables in young cochlear implant recipients and hearing children in the single-word period. J Speech Lang Hear Res 53 (01) 2-17
  • Wolfe J, Neumann S, Marsh M, Schafer E, Lianos L, Gilden J, O’Neill L, Arkis P, Menapace C, Nel E, Jones M. 2015; Benefits of adaptive signal processing in a commercially available cochlear implant sound processor. Otol Neurotol 36 (07) 1181-1190
  • Yoshinaga-Itano C, Baca RL, Sedey AL. 2010; Describing the trajectory of language development in the presence of severe-to-profound hearing loss: a closer look at children with cochlear implants versus hearing aids. Otol Neurotol 31 (08) 1268-1274
  • Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey AL, Coulter DK, Mehl AL. 1998; Language of early- and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics 102 (05) 1161-1171
  • Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey A. 2000; Early speech development in children who are deaf or hard of hearing: interrelationships with language and hearing. Volta Review 100: 181-211

Corresponding author

Rene Gifford
Vanderbilt University
Nashville, TN 37232

  • REFERENCES

  • Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) 2011 Technology assessment: effectiveness of cochlear implants in adults with sensorineural hearing loss. Contract no. 290 2007 10055 1). https://www.cms.gov/Medicare/Coverage/DeterminationProcess/downloads/id80TA.pdf Accessed August 1, 2016
  • Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) 2014 Methods guide for comparative effectiveness comparative reviews. Publication no. 10(14)-EHC063-EF. https://effectivehealthcare.ahrq.gov/ehc/products/60/318/CER-Methods-Guide-140109.pdf Accessed August 1, 2016
  • Akhtar N, Jipson J, Callanan MA. 2001; Learning words through overhearing. Child Dev 72 (02) 416-430
  • Ambrose SE, Berry LMU, Walker EA, Harrison M, Oleson J, Moeller MP. 2014; Speech sound production in 2-year-olds who are hard of hearing. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 23: 91-104
  • Baudhuin J, Cadieux J, Firszt JB, Reeder RM, Maxson JL. 2012; Optimization of programming parameters in children with the advanced bionics cochlear implant. J Am Acad Audiol 23 (05) 302-312
  • Bench J, Kowal A, Bamford J. 1979; The BKB (Bamford-Kowal-Bench) sentence lists for partially-hearing children. Br J Audiol 13 (03) 108-112
  • Bess FH, Sinclair JS, Riggs DE. 1984; Group amplification in schools for the hearing impaired. Ear Hear 5 (03) 138-144
  • Beyea JA, McMullen KP, Harris MS, Houston DM, Martin JM., Bolster VA, Adunka OF, Moberly AC. 2016; Cochlear implants in adults: effects of age and duration of deafness on speech recognition. Otol Neurotol 37 (09) 1238-1245
  • Boothroyd A. 1995. Speech perception tests and hearing impaired children. In: Plant G, Spens KE. Profound Deafness and Speech Communication. London, UK: Whurr;
  • Boothroyd A, Geers AE, Moog JS. 1991; Practical implications of cochlear implants in children. Ear Hear 12 (04) (Suppl) 81S-89S
  • Brand Y, Senn P, Kompis M, Dillier N, Allum JH. 2014; Cochlear implantation in children and adults in Switzerland. Swiss Med Wkly 144: w13909
  • Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services 2004 The CMS approved coverage with evidence development (CED) study protocol CAG-00107N, CMS. . https://www.cms.gov/medicare-coverage-database/details/nca-decision-memo.aspx?NCAId=134&NcaName=Cochlear+Implantation&NCDId=245&ncdver=2&IsPopup=y&bc=AAAAAAAAIAAA& December 21, 2016.
  • Ching TYC, Leigh G, Dillon H. 2013; Introduction to the longitudinal outcomes of children with hearing impairment (LOCHI) study: background, design, sample characteristics. Int J Audiol 52 (2, Suppl) S4-S9
  • Clark W, Govett S. 1995. School-related noise exposure in children . Paper presented at the Association for Research in in Otolaryngology Mid-Winter Meeting; St. Petersburg, FL:
  • Coninx F, Weichbold V, Tsiakpini L. et al. 2009; Validation of the LittlEARS((R)) Auditory Questionnaire in children with normal hearing. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 73 (12) 1761-1768
  • Crandell C, Smaldino J. 1995; An update of classroom acoustics for children with hearing impairment. Volta Review 1: 4-12
  • Crukley J, Scollie S, Parsa V. 2011; An exploration of non-quiet listening at school. J Ed Audiol 17: 23-35
  • Davidson LS. 2006; Effects of stimulus level on the speech perception abilities of children using cochlear implants or digital hearing aids. Am J Audiol 15 (02) 141-153
  • Davidson LS, Skinner MW, Holstad BA, Fears BT, Richter MK, Matusofsky M, Brenner C, Holden T, Birath A, Kettel JL, Scollie S. 2009; The effect of instantaneous input dynamic range setting on the speech perception of children with the nucleus 24 implant. Ear Hear 30 (03) 340-349
  • Desai S, Stickney G, Zeng FG. 2008; Auditory-visual speech perception in normal-hearing and cochlear-implant listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 123 (01) 428-440
  • Dorman MF, Cook S, Spahr A, Zhang T, Loiselle L, Schramm D, Whittingham J, Gifford R.. 2015; Factors constraining the benefit to speech understanding of combining information from low-frequency hearing and a cochlear implant. Hear Res 322: 107-111
  • Dorman M, Spahr A, Gifford RH, Cook S, Zhang T, Loiselle L, Whittingham J, Schramm D.. 2012; Bilateral and bimodal benefit as a function of age for adults. J Hear Sci 2 (04) 37-39
  • Dwyer RT, Spahr T, Agrawal S, Hetlinger C, Holder JT, Gifford RH. 2016; Participant-generated cochlear implant programs: speech recognition, sound quality, and satisfaction. Otol Neurotol 37 (07) e209-e216
  • Eilers RE, Wilson WR, Moore JM. 1977; Developmental changes in speech discrimination in infants. J Acoust Soc Am 20: 766-780
  • Eisenberg LS, Johnson KC, Martinez AS, Cokely CG, Tobey EA, Quittner AL, Fink NE, Wang NY, Niparko JK. CDaCI Investigative Team (2006) Speech recognition at 1-year follow-up in the childhood development after cochlear implantation study: methods and preliminary findings. Audiol Neurootol 11 (04) 259-268
  • Eisenberg LS, Martinez AS, Boothroyd A. 2003; Auditory-visual and auditory-only perception of phonetic contrasts in children. Volta Review 103: 327-346
  • Eisenberg LS, Boothroyd A, Martinez AS. 2004. Cochlear Implants International Congress Series. The perception of phonetic contrasts in infants: development of the VRASPAC. Vol. 1273, pp. 364-367 Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier;
  • Eshraghi AA, Nazarian R, Telischi FF, Martinez D, Hodges A, Velandia S, Cejas-Cruz I, Balkany TJ, Lo K, Lang D. 2015; Cochlear implantation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Otol Neurotol 36 (08) e121-e128
  • Etymōtic Research, Inc 2005 BKB-SIN test. Speech-in-Noise Test Version 1.03, 2005. 61 Martin Lane, Elk Grove Village, IL. www.etymotic.com/auditory-research/speech-in-noise-tests/bkb-sin.html Accessed December 21, 2016.
  • Eze N, Ofo E, Jiang D, O’Connor AF. 2013; Systematic review of cochlear implantation in children with developmental disability. Otol Neurotol 34 (08) 1385-1393
  • Fenson L, Marchman VA, Thal DJ, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E. 2007. MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual. 2nd ed. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing;
  • Finitzo-Hieber T (1988) Classroom acoustics. In: Roeser R. Auditory Disorders in School Children. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Thieme-Stratton; 221-223
  • Fink NE, Wang NY, Visaya J, Niparko JK, Quittner A, Eisenberg LS, Tobey EA. CDACI Investigative Team (2007) Childhood Development after Cochlear Implantation (CDaCI) study: design and baseline characteristics. Cochlear Implants Int 8 (02) 92-116
  • Firszt JB, Holden LK, Skinner MW, Tobey EA, Peterson A, Gaggl W, Runge-Samuelson CL, Wackym PA. 2004; Recognition of speech presented at soft to loud levels by adult cochlear implant recipients of three cochlear implant systems. Ear Hear 25 (04) 375-387
  • Fulcher A, Purcell AA, Baker E, Munro N. 2012; Listen up: children with early identified hearing loss achieve age-appropriate speech/language outcomes by 3 years-of-age. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 76 (12) 1785-1794
  • Ganger J, Brent MR. 2004; Reexamining the vocabulary spurt. Dev Psychol 40 (04) 621-632
  • Geers AE, Davidson LS, Uchanski RM, Nicholas JG. 2013; Interdependence of linguistic and indexical speech perception skills in school-age children with early cochlear implantation. Ear Hear 34 (05) 562-574
  • Gifford RH, Dorman MF, Sheffield SW, Teece K, Olund AP. 2014; Availability of binaural cues for bilateral implant recipients and bimodal listeners with and without preserved hearing in the implanted ear. Audiol Neurootol 19 (01) 57-71
  • Gifford RH, Olund AP, Dejong M. 2011; Improving speech perception in noise for children with cochlear implants. J Am Acad Audiol 22 (09) 623-632
  • Gifford RH, Shallop JK, Peterson AM. 2008; Speech recognition materials and ceiling effects: considerations for cochlear implant programs. Audiol Neurootol 13 (03) 193-205
  • Govaerts PJ, Daemers K, Yperman M, De Beukelaer C, De Saegher G, De Ceulaer G. 2006; Auditory speech sounds evaluation (A(section)E): a new test to assess detection, discrimination and identification in hearing impairment. Cochlear Implants Int 7 (02) 92-106
  • Govaerts PJ, Vaerenberg B, De Ceulaer G, Daemers K, De Beukelaer C, Schauwers K. 2010; Development of a software tool using deterministic logic for the optimization of cochlear implant processor programming. Otol Neurotol 31 (06) 908-918
  • Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. 2009; Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform 42 (02) 377-381
  • Hart TR, Risley B. 1995. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing;
  • Hayes H, Geers AE, Treiman R, Moog JS. 2009; Receptive vocabulary development in deaf children with cochlear implants: achievement in an intensive auditory-oral educational setting. Ear Hear 30 (01) 128-135
  • Hillock-Dunn A, Buss E, Duncan N, Roush PA, Leibold LJ. 2014; Effects of nonlinear frequency compression on speech identification in children with hearing loss. Ear Hear 35 (03) 353-365
  • Hillock-Dunn A, Taylor C, Buss E, Leibold LJ. 2015; Assessing speech perception in children with hearing loss: what conventional clinical tools may miss. Ear Hear 36 (02) e57-e60
  • Holte L, Walker E, Oleson J, Spratford M, Moeller MP, Roush P, Ou H, Tomblin JB. 2012; Factors influencing follow-up to newborn hearing screening for infants who are hard of hearing. Am J Audiol 21 (02) 163-174
  • Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Cymerman E, Levine S. 2002; Language input and child syntax. Cognit Psychol 45 (03) 337-374
  • Inscoe JR, Bones C. 2016; Additional difficulties associated with aetiologies of deafness: outcomes from a parent questionnaire of 540 children using cochlear implants. Cochlear Implants Int 17 (01) 21-30
  • Jerger S, Jerger J, Abrams S. 1983; Speech audiometry in the young child. Ear Hear 4 (01) 56-66
  • Kashinath S, Woods J, Goldstein H. 2006; Enhancing generalized teaching strategy use in daily routines by parents of children with autism. J Speech Lang Hear Res 49 (03) 466-485
  • Kent RD, Osberger MJ, Netsell R, Hustedde CG. 1987; Phonetic development in identical twins differing in auditory function. J Speech Hear Disord 52 (01) 64-75
  • Kirk KI, French B, Choi S. 2009. Assessing spoken word recognition in children with cochlear implants. In: Eisenberg LS. Clinical Management of Children with Cochlear Implants. San Diego, CA: Plural Publishers;
  • Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Osberger MJ. 1995; Lexical effects on spoken word recognition by pediatric cochlear implant users. Ear Hear 16 (05) 470-481
  • Kirk KI, Pisoni DB, Sommers MS, Young M, Evanson C. 1995; New directions for assessing speech perception in persons with sensory aids. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl 166: 300-303
  • Koch DB, Quick A, Osberger MJ, Saoji A, Litvak L. 2014; Enhanced hearing in noise for cochlear implant recipients: clinical trial results for a commercially available speech-enhancement strategy. Otol Neurotol 35 (05) 803-809
  • Koehlinger KM, Van Horne AJO, Moeller MP. 2013; Grammatical outcomes of 3- and 6-year-old children who are hard of hearing. J Speech Lang Hear Res 56 (05) 1701-1714
  • Kuehn-Inacken H, Weichboldt V, Tsiakpini L, Coninx F, D’Haese P. (2003) LittlEARS Auditory questionnaire: parents questionnaire to assess auditory behaviour. Innsbruck, Austria: MedEl.
  • Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR, Miller-Loncar CL. 2000; Early maternal and child influences on children’s later independent cognitive and social functioning. Child Dev 71 (02) 358-375
  • Law J, Rush R, Schoon I, Parsons S. 2009; Modeling developmental language difficulties from school entry into adulthood: literacy, mental health, and employment outcomes. J Speech Lang Hear Res 52 (06) 1401-1416
  • Leibold LJ, Hillock-Dunn A, Duncan N, Roush PA, Buss E. 2013; Influence of hearing loss on children’s identification of spondee words in a speech-shaped noise or a two-talker masker. Ear Hear 34 (05) 575-584
  • Olsen WO. 1998; Average speech levels and spectra in various speaking/listening conditions: a summary of the Pearson, Bennett, and Fidell (1977) report. Am J Audiol 7 (02) 21-25
  • Mahmoud AF, Ruckenstein MJ. 2014; Speech perception performance as a function of age at implantation among postlingually deaf adult cochlear implant recipients. Otol Neurotol 35 (10) e286-e291
  • Martinez A, Eisenberg L, Boothroyd A, Visser-Dumont L. 2008; Assessing speech pattern contrast perception in infants: Early results on VRASPAC. Otol Neuroto 29: 183-188
  • Massa ST, Ruckenstein MJ. 2014; Comparing the performance plateau in adult cochlear implant patients using HINT and AzBio. Otol Neurotol 35 (04) 598-604
  • Mayne A, Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey AL. 2000; Receptive vocabulary development of infants and toddlers who are deaf or hard of hearing. Volta Review 100: 29-52
  • Mayne AM, Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey AL, Carey A. 2000; Expressive vocabulary development of infants and toddlers who are deaf or hard of hearing. Volta Review 100: 1-28
  • McCreery RW, Alexander J, Brennan MA, Hoover B, Kopun J, Stelmachowicz PG. 2014; The influence of audibility on speech recognition with nonlinear frequency compression for children and adults with hearing loss. Ear Hear 35 (04) 440-447
  • McCreery RW, Walker EA, Spratford M, Oleson J, Bentler R, Holte L, Roush P. 2015; Speech recognition and parent ratings From auditory development questionnaires in children who are hard of hearing. Ear Hear 36 (1, Suppl) 60S-75S
  • McGuckian M, Henry A. 2007; The grammatical morpheme deficit in moderate hearing impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord 42 (1, Suppl) 17-36
  • McMurray B. 2007; Defusing the childhood vocabulary explosion. Science 317 (5838) 631
  • Meinzen-Derr J, Wiley S, Creighton J, Choo D. 2007; Auditory Skills Checklist: clinical tool for monitoring functional auditory skill development in young children with cochlear implants. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 116 (11) 812-818
  • Minimum-Speech-Test-Battery (MSTB) 2011 The new minimum speech test battery. . http://www.auditorypotential.com/MSTB_Nav.html Accessed December 21, 2016
  • Moeller MP, Hoover B, Putman C, Arbataitis K, Bohnenkamp G, Peterson B, Lewis D, Estee S, Pittman A, Stelmachowicz P. 2007; Vocalizations of infants with hearing loss compared with infants with normal hearing: part I—phonetic development. Ear Hear 28 (05) 605-627
  • Moeller MP, Hoover B, Putman C, Arbataitis K, Bohnenkamp G, Peterson B, Wood S, Lewis D, Pittman A, Stelmachowicz P. 2007; Vocalizations of infants with hearing loss compared with infants with normal hearing: part II—transition to words. Ear Hear 28 (05) 628-642
  • Moog J, Geers A. 1990. Early Speech Perception Test. St. Louis, MO: Central Institute for the Deaf;
  • Moore J, Thompson G, Thompson M. 1975; Auditory localization of infants as a function of reinforcement conditions. J Speech Lang Hear Dis 40: 29-34
  • Niparko JK, Tobey EA, Thal DJ, Eisenberg LS, Wang NY, Quittner AL, Fink NE. CDaCI Investigative Team (2010) Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. JAMA 303 (15) 498-506
  • Nober L, Nober E. 1975; Auditory discrimination of learning disabled children in quiet and classroom noise. J Learn Disabil 8: 656-659
  • Nozza RJ. 1987; Infant speech-sound discrimination testing: Effects of stimulus intensity and procedural model on measures of performance. J Acoust Soc Am 81: 1928-1939
  • Olds C, Pollonini L, Abaya H, Larky J, Loy M, Bortfeld H, Beauchamp MS, Oghalai JS. 2016; Cortical activation patterns correlate with speech understanding after cochlear implantation. Ear Hear 37 (03) e160-e172
  • Oller DK, Eilers RE. 1988; The role of audition in infant babbling. Child Dev 59 (02) 441-449
  • Osberger MJ, Miyamoto RT, Zimmerman-Phillips S, Kemink JL, Stroer BS, Firszt JB, Novak MA. 1991; Independent evaluation of the speech perception abilities of children with the Nucleus 22-channel cochlear implant system. Ear Hear 12 (04) (Suppl) 66S-80S
  • Pearsons K, Bennett R, Fidell S. 1977. Speech Levels in Various Environments. Washington, DC: Office of Health and Ecological Effects, Office of Research and Development, US EPA:
  • Pediatric Minimum Speech Test Battery Working Group (PMSTB) 2013 Meeting minutes, project updates and file cabinets. . https://sites.google.com/site/pediatricmstbworkinggroup/home Accessed December 21, 2016
  • Peterson GE, Lehiste I. 1962; Revised CNC lists for auditory tests. J Speech Hear Disord 27: 62-70
  • Rakszawski B, Wright R, Cadieux JH, Davidson LS, Brenner C. 2016; The effects of preprocessing strategies for pediatric cochlear implant recipients. J Am Acad Audiol 27 (02) 85-102
  • Robbins AM, Kirk KI. 1996; Speech perception assessment and performance in pediatric cochlear implant users. Semin Hear 17: 353-369
  • Robinson EJ, Davidson LS, Uchanski RM, Brenner CM, Geers AE. 2012; A longitudinal study of speech perception skills and device characteristics of adolescent cochlear implant users. J Am Acad Audiol 23 (05) 341-349
  • Roeser R, Clark J. 2008; Live voice speech recognition audiometry: stop the madness. Audiol Today 20: 32-33
  • Roland Jr JT, Gantz BJ, Waltzman SB, Parkinson AJ. Multicenter Clinical Trial Group (2016) United States multicenter clinical trial of the cochlear nucleus hybrid implant system. Laryngoscope 126 (01) 175-181
  • Runge CL, Henion K, Tarima S, Beiter A, Zwolan TA. 2016; Clinical outcomes of the Cochlear™ Nucleus(®) 5 Cochlear Implant System and SmartSound™ 2 Signal Processing. J Am Acad Audiol 27 (06) 425-440
  • Sanders DA. 1965; Noise conditions in normal school classrooms. Except Child 31: 344-353
  • Sarant JZ, Holt CM, Dowell RC, Rickards FW, Blamey PJ. 2009; Spoken language development in oral preschool children with permanent childhood deafness. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 14 (02) 205-217
  • Schafer EC. 2010; Speech perception in noise measures for children: a critical review and case studies. J. Ed Audiol 16: 4-15
  • Sheffield SW, Haynes DS, Wanna GB, Labadie RF, Gifford RH. 2015; Availability of binaural cues for pediatric bilateral cochlear implant recipients. J Am Acad Audiol 26 (03) 289-298
  • Skinner MW, Holden LK, Holden TA, Demorest ME. 1999; Comparison of two methods for selecting minimum stimulation levels used in programming the Nucleus 22 cochlear implant. J Speech Lang Hear Res 42 (04) 814-828
  • Smeds K, Wolters F, Rung M. 2015; Estimation of signal-to-noise ratio in realistic sound scenarios. J Am Acad Audiol 26 (02) 183-196
  • Spahr AJ, Dorman MF, Litvak LM, Cook SJ, Loiselle LM, DeJong MD, Hedley-Williams A, Sunderhaus LS, Hayes CA, Gifford RH. 2014; Development and validation of the pediatric AzBio sentence lists. Ear Hear 35 (04) 418-422
  • Spahr AJ, Dorman MF, Loiselle LH. 2007; Performance of patients using different cochlear implant systems: effects of input dynamic range. Ear Hear 28 (02) 260-275
  • Stelmachowicz P, Lewis D, Hoover B, Nishi K, McCreery R, Woods W. 2010; Effects of digital noise reduction on speech perception for children with hearing loss. Ear Hear 31 (03) 345-355
  • Stelmachowicz PG, Pittman AL, Hoover BM, Lewis DE. 2002; Aided perception of /s/ and /z/ by hearing-impaired children. Ear Hear 23 (04) 316-324
  • Stoel-Gammon C, Otomo K. 1986; Babbling development of hearing-impaired and normally hearing subjects. J Speech Hear Disord 51 (01) 33-41
  • Suskind D, Leffel KR, Hernandez MW, Sapolich SG, Suskind E, Kirkham E, Meehan P. 2013; An exploratory study of “quantitative linguistic feedback”: effect of LENA feedback on adult language production. Comm Disord Q 34 (04) 199-209
  • Tobey EA, Geers AE, Brenner C, Altuna D, Gabbert G. 2003; Factors associated with development of speech production skills in children implanted by age five. Ear Hear 24 (01) (Suppl) 36S-45S
  • Tobey EA, Geers AE, Sundarrajan M, Shin S. 2011; Factors influencing speech production in elementary and high school-aged cochlear implant users. Ear Hear 32 (01) (Suppl) 27S-38S
  • Tobey EA, Thal D, Niparko JK, Eisenberg LS, Quittner AL, Wang NY. CDaCI Investigative Team (2013) Influence of implantation age on school-age language performance in pediatric cochlear implant users. Int J Audiol 52 (04) 219-229
  • Tomblin JB, Oleson JJ, Ambrose SE, Walker E, Moeller MP. 2014; The influence of hearing aids on the speech and language development of children with hearing loss. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 140 (05) 403-409
  • Tomblin JB, Spencer L, Flock S, Tyler R, Gantz B. 1999; A comparison of language achievement in children with cochlear implants and children using hearing aids. J Speech Lang Hear Res 42 (02) 497-509
  • Tomblin JB, Walker EA, McCreery RW, Arenas RM, Harrison M, Moeller MP. 2015; Outcomes of children with hearing loss: data collection and methods. Ear Hear 36 (1, Suppl 1) 14S-23S
  • Tyler RS, Berliner KI, Demorest ME, Hirshorn MS, Luxford WM, Mangham CA. 1986; Clinical objectives and research design issues for cochlear implants in children. Semin Hear 7: 433-440
  • Tyler RS, Davis JM, Lansing CR. 1987; Cochlear implants in young children. ASHA 29 (04) 41-49
  • Uhler K. 2014 Relationship between speech perception and language. Ultimate Colorado Midwinter Meeting, Vail, CO, February 2014
  • Uhler KM, Baca R, Dudas E, Fredrickson T. 2015; Refining stimulus parameters in assessing infant speech perception using visual reinforcement infant speech discrimination: sensation level. J Am Acad Audiol 26 (10) 807-814
  • Uhler K, Biever A, Gifford RH. 2016; Method of speech stimulus presentation impacts pediatric speech recognition: monitored live voice versus recorded speech. Otol Neurotol 37 (02) e70-e74
  • Uhler K, Burns S, Dalpes M, Yoshinaga-Itano C (2011) An auditory spoken language matrix for differential diagnosis of Spanish-speaking children who are deaf or hard of hearing. ASHA Spec Interes Group 1418:71–78. doi: 10.1044/cds18.3.71
  • Uhler K, Gifford RH. 2014; Current trends in pediatric cochlear implant candidate selection and postoperative follow-up. Am J Audiol 23 (03) 309-325
  • Waltzman S, Cohen NL, Spivak L, Ying E, Brackett D, Shapiro W, Hoffman R. 1990; Improvement in speech perception and production abilities in children using a multichannel cochlear implant. Laryngoscope 100 (03) 240-243
  • Wang NY, Eisenberg LS, Johnson KC, Fink NE, Tobey EA, Quittner AL, Niparko JK. CDaCI Investigative Team (2008) Tracking development of speech recognition: longitudinal data from hierarchical assessments in the Childhood Development after Cochlear Implantation Study. Otol Neurotol 29 (02) 240-245
  • Warner-Czyz AD, Davis BL. 2008; The emergence of segmental accuracy in young cochlear implant recipients. Cochlear Implants Int 9 (03) 143-166
  • Warner-Czyz AD, Davis BL, MacNeilage PF. 2010; Accuracy of consonant-vowel syllables in young cochlear implant recipients and hearing children in the single-word period. J Speech Lang Hear Res 53 (01) 2-17
  • Wolfe J, Neumann S, Marsh M, Schafer E, Lianos L, Gilden J, O’Neill L, Arkis P, Menapace C, Nel E, Jones M. 2015; Benefits of adaptive signal processing in a commercially available cochlear implant sound processor. Otol Neurotol 36 (07) 1181-1190
  • Yoshinaga-Itano C, Baca RL, Sedey AL. 2010; Describing the trajectory of language development in the presence of severe-to-profound hearing loss: a closer look at children with cochlear implants versus hearing aids. Otol Neurotol 31 (08) 1268-1274
  • Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey AL, Coulter DK, Mehl AL. 1998; Language of early- and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics 102 (05) 1161-1171
  • Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey A. 2000; Early speech development in children who are deaf or hard of hearing: interrelationships with language and hearing. Volta Review 100: 181-211

Zoom Image