``Pay attention!'' says the teacher to a daydreaming student. ``Are you paying attention?''
asks the wife who's reviewing the weekend plans with her seemingly distracted husband.
``Atten-shun!'' shouts the lieutenant to his troops. ``I wasn't paying attention,''
explains the motorist who backed into a fence. These examples serve to highlight the
practical, everyday importance of attention. It is an often-mentioned human behavior
because we all seem to appreciate that processing of any form of environmental information
requires us to attend to that information. Most ``accidents'' are caused by failures
or decrements of attention. This fact is now well-documented by studies of motorists
using cellular telephones. Furthermore, attention is basic to all learning. As attention
for new, incoming stimuli decreases, learning decreases. Clinicians recognize that
all forms of therapy involve some form of learning. If the individual receiving treatment
is not relegating the amount and type of attention required by a task, then incomplete,
incorrect, or even no learning takes place. We also recognize that tests of other
domains of cognition, such as language and executive skills can yield invalid assessments
of those skills secondary to attentional deficits.
Clearly, the cognitive domain of attention is one of critical importance to purposeful
human activity. According to Parasuraman,[1] however, ``Attention is not a single entity, but the name given to a finite set
of brain processes that can interact, mutually and with other brain processes, in
the performance of different perceptual, cognitive, and motor tasks.'' To accurately
assess ``attention'' and successfully treat disorders of attention, therefore, clinicians
must recognize and understand all attentional processes. To help clarify these concepts,
we decided to dedicate a volume of Seminars in Speech and Language to this topic. To serve as Guest Editor for this volume, I called upon Dr. Laura
Murray, because I am very familiar with her excellent work in the realm of attention
and aphasia. Dr. Murray graciously agreed to the task and put together a team of contributors
to address the full range of considerations, including descriptions of forms of attention,
the neuroanatomy of attention, the neuropathology of attention disorders in specific
populations of brain-damaged adults, and the assessment and treatment of these disorders.
As readers will learn, there is quite a bit more to attention than is generally appreciated,
but even an understanding of various normal attentional processes is not sufficient
for optimal clinical practices. It is also important to be alert to the types of attentional
problems most likely to occur as a result of various neurological disorders such as
left hemisphere and right hemisphere strokes, traumatic brain injury and Alzheimer's
disease. Then, clinicians must be prepared to evaluate and differentially diagnose
attentional impairments so as to choose or develop appropriate management and therapy
techniques. I am confident that this volume will provide readers with information
for improving their clinical practices vis-à-vis the cognitive domain of attention.
Nancy Helm-Estabrooks, Sc.D.
Co-Editor in Chief