Subscribe to RSS

DOI: 10.1055/s-0045-1809926
When You Don't Have Chronotype Data: Sleep Questionnaires as a Circadian Window
Funding Source This research received no external funding.
Although closely related to sleep, circadian rest-activity rhythms represent an independent dimension of human physiological and behavioral functioning, and disruptions in these rhythms have been associated with adverse physical and mental health outcomes across the lifespan.[1] However, assessing circadian rhythms is methodologically complex, often requiring extended actigraphic monitoring or laboratory-based methods such as salivary melatonin sampling to estimate dim light melatonin onset (DLMO).[2] [3] To address this, constructs like chronotype – defined as an individual's preferred timing for activity and rest – have been operationalized,[4] enabling the development of self-report measures widely used in sleep medicine and chronobiology, such as the morningness-eveningness questionnaire (MEQ) and the Munich chronotype questionnaire (MCTQ).[5] [6] [7]
From a theoretical standpoint, it is crucial to distinguish between biological circadian rhythms – measurable through markers such as DLMO or core body temperature[8] under free-running conditions, where external synchronizers are minimized – and real-world, socially regulated daily rhythms. The latter reflects the interplay between endogenous predispositions and external factors such as work schedules, artificial light exposure, geographic latitude, and cultural habits. Accordingly, measures like sleep timing preferences or chronotype estimates from self-report questionnaires capture a ‘manifested’ rhythm rather than a direct readout of the circadian phase and should be interpreted with these contextual influences in mind.
Furthermore, while chronotype assessments have proved valuable for exploring links between circadian preference and health outcomes, most large-scale population studies lack these measures, thereby limiting the ability to examine circadian factors in epidemiological research.[1]
As a partial remedy, it may be promising to identify proxies of circadian preference within existing sleep questionnaires. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI),[9] a well-established self-report measure of sleep quality, includes items assessing habitual sleep and wake times over the past month, and similar information is also captured in large-scale surveys such as the Centers for Disease Control's National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES).[10] Though such timing data correlate only modestly with biological circadian markers like DLMO,[2] questionnaire-derived estimates of sleep onset (bedtime + sleep latency), sleep offset (wake-up time), and sleep midpoint (the average of sleep onset and offset) may still provide unique circadian information beyond standard subjective sleep parameters (see [Fig. 1]).


In support of this approach, we examined previously collected data from 1,234 university students (mean age = 23.3 ± 2.4 years; 87.3% women, 12% men, 0.7% undisclosed). Sleep onset, offset, and midpoint calculated from self-reported habitual bedtime, sleep latency, and wake-up time, were significantly but only moderately correlated with sleep duration (r = -.23, r = .34, and r = .06, respectively; all p-values < .05). In multiple linear regression models controlling for sleep duration, sleep latency, and subjective sleep quality, sleep midpoint (selected as the most representative circadian proxy) was significantly associated with mental health-related quality of life (HRQoL) (β = -0.13, p = .004), but not with physical HRQoL (β = –0.02, p = .60). Furthermore, after classifying participants into early, intermediate, and late chronotype groups based on the 33rd and 66th percentiles of sleep midpoint, ANOVAs showed that late chronotypes reported significantly lower mental HRQoL compared to both early and intermediate chronotypes (F = 8.5, p < .001; post-hoc p-values < .001 and .02), and lower physical HRQoL compared to intermediates (F = 4.7, p = .01; post-hoc p = .007).
We propose that circadian proxies derived from instruments like the PSQI or other sleep timing questions are worth reporting and investigating when exploring associations between sleep and health outcomes, especially in large datasets lacking direct chronotype or objective circadian data. While such proxies do not capture the intrinsic circadian phase, they reflect socially expressed patterns of sleep-wake behavior – manifestations of circadian preference shaped by real-life constraints – which remain highly relevant for both research and public health purposes. Building on this approach, similar strategies may be applicable to other self-report instruments, and prospective sleep diaries may be particularly promising for estimating subjective circadian timing and its variability over longer intervals.
Conflicts of Interest
The author declares no conflicts of interest.
Ethical Committee Permission and Informed Consent
The data collected from human participants reported in this study were detailed in previous publications. All procedures were approved by the competent Ethics Committee at Sapienza University of Rome (protocol number 0000/2021). All participants provided informed consent.
-
References
- 1 Meyer N, Harvey AG, Lockley SW, Dijk DJ. Circadian rhythms and disorders of the timing of sleep. Lancet 2022; 400 (10357): 1061-1078
- 2 Reiter AM, Sargent C, Roach GD. Finding DLMO: estimating dim light melatonin onset from sleep markers derived from questionnaires, diaries and actigraphy. Chronobiol Int 2020; 37 (9-10): 1412-1424
- 3 Pandi-Perumal SR, Smits M, Spence W. et al. Dim light melatonin onset (DLMO): a tool for the analysis of circadian phase in human sleep and chronobiological disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2007; 31 (01) 1-11
- 4 Roenneberg T. What is chronotype?: Preface. Sleep Biol Rhythms 2012; 10 (02) 75-76
- 5 Kivelä L, Papadopoulos MR, Antypa N. Chronotype and Psychiatric Disorders. Curr Sleep Med Rep 2018; 4 (02) 94-103
- 6 Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 1976; 4 (02) 97-110
- 7 Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms 2003; 18 (01) 80-90
- 8 Weinert D, Waterhouse J. The circadian rhythm of core temperature: effects of physical activity and aging. Physiol Behav 2007; 90 (2-3): 246-256
- 9 Buysse DJ, Reynolds III CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989; 28 (02) 193-213
- 10 Luo Z, Wang T, Wu W, Yan S, Chen L. Association between weekend catch-up sleep and depressive symptoms in American adults: Finding from NHANES 2017-2020. J Affect Disord 2024; 354: 36-43
Address for correspondence
Publication History
Received: 14 April 2025
Accepted: 05 June 2025
Article published online:
04 August 2025
© 2025. Brazilian Sleep Academy. This is an open access article published by Thieme under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonDerivative-NonCommercial License, permitting copying and reproduction so long as the original work is given appropriate credit. Contents may not be used for commercial purposes, or adapted, remixed, transformed or built upon. (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/)
Thieme Revinter Publicações Ltda.
Rua Rego Freitas, 175, loja 1, República, São Paulo, SP, CEP 01220-010, Brazil
Matteo Carpi. When You Don't Have Chronotype Data: Sleep Questionnaires as a Circadian Window. Sleep Sci ; : s00451809926.
DOI: 10.1055/s-0045-1809926
-
References
- 1 Meyer N, Harvey AG, Lockley SW, Dijk DJ. Circadian rhythms and disorders of the timing of sleep. Lancet 2022; 400 (10357): 1061-1078
- 2 Reiter AM, Sargent C, Roach GD. Finding DLMO: estimating dim light melatonin onset from sleep markers derived from questionnaires, diaries and actigraphy. Chronobiol Int 2020; 37 (9-10): 1412-1424
- 3 Pandi-Perumal SR, Smits M, Spence W. et al. Dim light melatonin onset (DLMO): a tool for the analysis of circadian phase in human sleep and chronobiological disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2007; 31 (01) 1-11
- 4 Roenneberg T. What is chronotype?: Preface. Sleep Biol Rhythms 2012; 10 (02) 75-76
- 5 Kivelä L, Papadopoulos MR, Antypa N. Chronotype and Psychiatric Disorders. Curr Sleep Med Rep 2018; 4 (02) 94-103
- 6 Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 1976; 4 (02) 97-110
- 7 Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms 2003; 18 (01) 80-90
- 8 Weinert D, Waterhouse J. The circadian rhythm of core temperature: effects of physical activity and aging. Physiol Behav 2007; 90 (2-3): 246-256
- 9 Buysse DJ, Reynolds III CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989; 28 (02) 193-213
- 10 Luo Z, Wang T, Wu W, Yan S, Chen L. Association between weekend catch-up sleep and depressive symptoms in American adults: Finding from NHANES 2017-2020. J Affect Disord 2024; 354: 36-43

