Subscribe to RSS

DOI: 10.1055/s-0045-1804518
Auditory Processing in Stuttering Children: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Assessments
Funding The authors declare that the present work was supported by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES), Brazil.
Abstract
Introduction Stuttering is a human communication disorder characterized by involuntary interruptions to speech flow. Electrophysiological tests and behavioral evaluations allow the neuroaudiological processes involved in stuttering to be investigated.
Objective To investigate group differences in the central auditory system using long-latency auditory evoked potentials, cognitive potentials, and behavioral assessments in children who stutter and compare them with fluent children.
Methods We assessed 18 children with stuttering and compared them with a control group of 18 children without speech or language impairment. All children were evaluated with pure tone and speech audiometry, acoustic immittance measures, brainstem auditory evoked potentials, long latency auditory evoked potentials, cognitive potentials, and behavioral tests of auditory processing – Random Gap Detection Test (RGDT), Dichotic Digit Test (DDT); Duration Pattern Test (DPT); Pediatric Speech Intelligibility (PSI); and Masking Level Difference (MLD). The Scale of Auditory Behaviors (SAB) questionnaire was also applied.
Results Children who stuttered had longer latencies of the P2 and P3 waves compared with the fluent group. There was no difference in P2 amplitudes, but there was a smaller P3 amplitude in children who stuttered, and they also showed significant alterations in the DDT and DPT. Furthermore, an association was found between increased P2 and P3 latency and SAB scores.
Conclusion The present study indicates that children who stutter tend to have decreased auditory ability in terms of central auditory processing, and this shows up psychophysically and on the SAB questionnaire.
Keywords
stuttering - event-related potentials - P300 - hearing - children - auditory evoked potentialsIntroduction
Stuttering is a disorder characterized by involuntary interruptions that make it difficult to speak fluidly. There are repetitions, prolongations, and blocks, generally occupying more than 3% of speech.[1] [2] [3] It is common for it to begin in childhood, even before 18 months of age. Around 95% of children develop the disorder before the age of 4, with the peak age being around 33 months.[4]
The prevalence of stuttering is estimated to range from 2.2 to 5.6% of the general population,[4] while in preschool-aged children the incidence ranges from 5 to 11%.[5] [6] As with other language and speech disorders, males are most affected. In children under five years old, the proportion of boys who stutter is twice as high as that of girls. The difference between the sexes becomes even more pronounced with age, with an increasing proportion of males.[4] [7]
The cause of stuttering is multifactorial and is linked to neurophysiological and genetic factors. Studies have shown anatomical variations, morphological differences, hemispheric asymmetry, and difficulties in motor, linguistic, and auditory function.[8] [9] [10] [11] [12] [13] [14] [15]
Speech perception and production are related, and the production of intelligible speech depends on the ability to follow the paradigms of the acoustic spectrum and understand the prosody of the interlocutor's speech.[16] [17] Thus, research in this area involves investigating the relationship between auditory function and language disorders, including stuttering. Studies focused on stuttering and hearing have found that, comparing fluent and disfluent individuals, there are distinct differences in both speech processing and the processing of auditory information.[18] [19] [20] [21] [22] [23] Nevertheless, studies that provide hard physiological evidence are scarce.
One way to objectively verify auditory integrity and functionality is through use of Auditory Evoked Potentials (AEPs), which are traces generated by the bioelectrical activity of the auditory pathways following acoustic stimulation.[24] [25] [26] [27] [28] Auditory evoked potential is an objective method that provides qualitative and quantitative information about the auditory pathway through measurement of the latency and amplitude of wave peaks. Measurements can, for example, determine the time it takes for an acoustic signal to travel through the auditory pathway and whether a stimulus is adequately received and interpreted by the auditory cortex.[26]
Long-latency auditory evoked potentials (LAEPs) refer to a series of electrical changes that occur in the central nervous system hundreds of milliseconds after stimulation. The bioelectrical responses of the thalamus and cortex are detected in the form of waves, with the sequence P1-N1-P2-N2 being recorded.[27] These exogenous potentials reflect auditory sensitivity at each level, providing information about the integrity of the auditory pathway, neural coding, and perception of acoustic stimuli at the central level.[28] [29] Some authors have used the P2 potential as a biomarker for auditory training, as a measure of the subject's auditory discrimination.[30] P2 is a prerequisite for the subsequent generation of the P3 wave, because a target stimulus must first be discriminated in order to be identified.[31] Thus, the study of P2 can help us understand the precognitive processes elicited by non-linguistic tones.
P3 is a LAEP that can be used to electrophysiologically assess hearing processing in the brain, as it reflects cortical activities involved in auditory processing.[24] [32] [33] [34] It is an endogenous potential that can also be called a cognitive potential, as in order for an individual to perform a cognitive task, an active response is first required, such as counting stimuli.[33] [34] P3 is a positive peak that appears around 300 milliseconds after active detection of a rare stimulus among frequent stimuli.[24] [27] It is a potential that reflects central auditory processes such as auditory discrimination, temporal processing, attention, discrimination, recognition, perception, and auditory memory.[24] [27] [34] [35] Its site of generation has yet to be completely determined, but it is known that the hippocampus, auditory cortex, prefrontal cortex, and centroparietal cortex contribute.[24]
There has been some research investigating the relationship between stuttering and P3, but no consensus has yet emerged. Whereas some studies[36] [37] [38] did not find any differences between stutterers and fluent speakers, others suggest there is some sort of change in the P3 wave in individuals who stutter –either an increase in latency[19] [36] [39] [40] [41] or a decrease in amplitude.[40] [42] The variability of results can be justified by the sample size, the heterogeneity of the samples, and the fact that most studies are performed with adults, which makes it difficult to assess whether the altered P3 results are the cause or consequence of stuttering.
Given this context, studies are necessary to better understand these responses, to contribute to scientific research and, in the future, with their use in clinical practice, with the aim of assisting in diagnosis and the development of therapeutic programs.
The aim of the present study is to record and analyze LAEPs–waves P2 and P3–and to correlate them with the behavioral assessments of children who stutter, comparing them with responses from fluent children. More generally, we also investigate whether there are any associations between the electrophysiological findings and stuttering severity or between electrophysiology and scores on the Scale of Auditory Behaviors questionnaire.
Methods
Subjects
This is a cross-sectional, analytical, and quantitative study. The convenience sample consisted of children aged 7 to 11 years old, of both sexes, divided into 2 groups: an experimental group (EG), consisting of children with developmental mild-to-severe stuttering, and a control group (CG) comprised of children without stuttering, matched by sex and age.
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
The EG was made up of children who spoke Brazilian Portuguese who had complaints of stuttering—but had received no prior treatment—and who had no comorbidities. We excluded those who did not know how to count from 1 to 50, had some type of hearing loss, an altered otorhinolaryngological evaluation, or who failed to perform the proposed procedures.
The CG was made up of children who spoke Brazilian Portuguese and had no history of psychological/psychiatric, neurological, motor, or communication disorders. We excluded those with hearing or language impairment, those who did not know how to count from 1 to 50, had some type of hearing loss, an altered otorhinolaryngological evaluation, or failed to perform the proposed procedures.
Equipment
-
a) Anamnesis. Information comprising age, sex, dominant hand, and education was collected with a description of any linguistic, social, neural, or otological impairment.
-
b) Assessment of stuttering. To assess the stuttering of the EG patients, an anamnesis of the child was performed, which verified family history regarding stuttering, its emergence, self-perception of the degree of disfluency, feelings about one's own speech, impact of stuttering on life, and daily routine. A speech sample was also collected, with video footage, consisting of spontaneous speech lasting ∼ 5 minutes. The speech was transcribed by the Speech Transcription Protocol,[43] and 200 syllables were analyzed by the Speech Fluency Assessment Protocol,[44] which evaluates the type of disfluencies, speech speed, frequency of ruptures, and physical parameters. Afterwards, the Stuttering Severity Instrument (SSI-4)[45] was applied to classify the severity of stuttering. Finally, a diagnosis of persistent developmental stuttering was made by two speech therapists with experience in the area. To test the agreement of the analysis between the judges, the Cohen's Kappa coefficient was used.
-
c) Peripheral audiological assessment
-
c.1) Children from both groups underwent inspection of the external auditory canal, tonal and speech audiometry, and acoustic immittance measurements. Inspection of the external auditory canal was performed with a otoscope (Welch Allyn, Inc., Skaneateles Falls, NY, USA) to rule out the presence of earwax or foreign body.
-
c.2) Pure tone audiometry was performed in an acoustically treated cabin, using a Harp audiometer (Inventis), with headphones and a bone vibrator. Hearing thresholds for frequencies from 0.25 to 8 kHz were investigated via air, while those from 0.5 to 4 kHz were investigated via bone. In speech audiometry, the Word Recognition Score (WRS) and Speech Recognition Threshold (SRT) were assessed, both relying on the patient repeating the words presented. To perform the WRS, 25 monosyllabic words were presented at an intensity of 40 dB re.
-
c.3) Acoustic immittance measurements were performed using an AT235h audiometer (Interacoustics A/S, Middelfart, Denmark). Ipsilateral and contralateral acoustic reflexes were investigated at 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz in both ears.
-
All children showed good ability to detect pure tones and had adequate sound transmission through the tympanic-ossicular system, with hearing thresholds better than 15 dB HL at frequencies from 0.25 to 8 kHz, type-A tympanometric curves, and ipsilateral and contralateral acoustic reflexes.
-
d) Behavioral tests of central auditory processing skill. These tests were performed in a soundproof booth, using verbal and non-verbal stimuli presented through headphones connected via the Harp audiometer to a notebook containing the behavioral test tracks. The test battery included the: (d.1) Random Gap Detection Test (RGDT); (d.2) Dichotic Digit Test (DDT); (d.3) Duration Pattern Test (DPT); (d.4) Pediatric Speech Intelligibility (PSI); and (d.5) Masking Level Difference (MLD), following the recommendations of Academia Brasileira de Audiologia.[46] The presentation intensity for all tests was calculated based on the mean air-conduction thresholds at 0.5, 1, and 2 kHz. All tests were performed after the children had been previously trained, ensuring they understood the tasks. For each test, the results were classified as normal or altered, following the standard normality values.
-
(d.1) In the RGDT, pure tones were presented paired with short intervals of silence, ranging from 0 to 40 milliseconds, in random order, and presented binaurally. Children had to report how many sounds they heard. The interval detection threshold was the smallest interval from which the child began to consistently identify the occurrence of two stimuli.
-
(d.2) The DDT was applied in two stages, binaural separation and integration. In the binaural integration stage, the child heard four numbers, two in each ear simultaneously, and they had to repeat all four digits, regardless of the order of presentation. In the binaural separation stage, two digits were presented in each ear, and the child was asked to repeat only the two digits heard in the selected ear, ignoring the digits in the contralateral ear. Each incorrectly identified digit was equivalent to a 1.25% error, and the number of errors × 1.25 was subtracted from 100%.
-
(d.3) In the DPT, 3 1-kHz tones of 2 lengths were presented at intervals of 300 milliseconds. The long tone (L) had a duration of 500 milliseconds and the short one (S) of 250 milliseconds. There were six patterns (LLS, SSL, LSL, SLS, LSS, SLL), which were repeated randomly. The child was asked to repeat the sequence, either naming or imitating it.
-
(d.4) In the PSI, the child was given a board containing pictures. Initially, the test pictures were presented for recognition only, and then, later, the child was instructed to pay attention and point to the picture corresponding to the sentence they heard, ignoring a competing message (a story). To begin, the test was applied in the presence of a contralateral competing message in the right ear (at speech/noise ratios of 0 dB and -40 dB) and later in the left ear at the same ratios. In the second stage, the test was applied with an ipsilateral competing message in the right ear (at speech/noise ratios of 0, -10, and -15 dB), and then in the left ear at the same ratios.
-
(d.5) In the MLD, 33 segments of narrowband noise were presented to one ear, for at least 3 seconds, in the presence or absence of a 0.5-kHz pure tone. Three conditions were considered: narrowband noise and pure tone in phase in both ears, pure tone in inverted phase in one ear, and noise in phase in both ears, and noise without the presence of the 0.5-kHz tone. The children were instructed to report every time they heard the tone. For analysis, the number of times that the child signaled they heard the tone were added up. This number was transformed into dB, according to a table supplied with the test.
-
-
e) Electrophysiological assessment
-
(e.1) Auditory Brainstem Response (ABR). A click stimulus was applied to verify the integrity of the auditory pathway. A click stimulus was used on the right and left ears separately. There were 27.7 clicks per second, of rarefied polarity and at 80 dB HL, to a total of 2,048 clicks, a recording window of 12 milliseconds, bandpass filter of 0.1 to 3 kHz, and digitization rate of 100 K. Participants were instructed to remain relaxed in an armchair, without speaking and with their eyes closed.
-
(e.2) Long latency brainstem auditory evoked potential. For this, the Masbe ATC Plus equipment (Contronic LLC, Huntington Beach, CA, USA) with a 3A earphone (Etymoic Research, Inc., Elk Grove Village, IL, USA) insert was used. The child was seated comfortably in a chair, and their skin was cleaned with exfoliant (Nuprep – Weaver and Company, Aurora, CO, USA) and gauze. Electrodes were then placed with conductive paste (Ten20–Weaver and Company) and secured with adhesive tape. The ground electrode was placed on the forehead, the active electrode (Fz) close to the scalp, the M1 electrode on the right mastoid, and the M2 electrode on the left. The earphones were placed in both ears. Impedance values were kept below 5Ω, and the difference between the 3 electrodes did not exceed 2Ω. An electroencephalogram (EEG) scan was performed to capture spontaneous brain electrical activity and check for artifacts that might interfere with the examination. Children were instructed not to tense their limbs and not to cross their legs and arms.
-
Tone burst stimuli of 1 or 2 kHz were applied at 80 dB HL. The tone bursts had a 20-millisecond plateau and 5 milliseconds rise and fall times. Each ear was tested individually. For recording P3, rare and frequent stimuli were presented. The child was instructed to pay attention and count rare stimuli, which appeared randomly among a series of frequent stimuli. At the end, the child needed to report how many rare stimuli they had heard. The frequent stimulus had a frequency of 1 kHz, and the rare stimulus 2 kHz, both presented randomly by the computer at a speed of 1.1 stimuli per second, in a rare-frequent (oddball) paradigm, with probabilities of appearance of 20% and 80%, respectively. A filter from 0.5 to 20 Hz and window of 900 milliseconds were used. The amplitude and latency values were obtained by looking at the two traces and identifying the highest peak. To identify P3, the largest positive polarity peak after the N1-P2-N2 complex was considered. The P3 component was identified by subtracting the rare-stimulus from the frequent-stimulus tracing. The exam was performed twice to verify reproducibility. These assessments followed the protocol described by Souza et al.,[47] Didoné et al.,[48] and McPherson.[27] The normality criteria were those proposed by McPherson.[27]
Finally, three speech therapists with experience in evaluating auditory potentials and P3 auditory evoked potentials independently analyzed the results.
-
f) Scale of Auditory Behaviors (SAB). With the help of their guardians, children from both groups answered the Scale of Auditory Behaviors (SAB) questionnaire, translated into European Portuguese by Nunes, Pereira, and Carvalho,[49] to assess the child's communication deficits and the impact on their daily life.
Statistical Analysis
A Shapiro-Wilk test was used to assess data normality. To verify the agreement of the analysis of the P2 and P3 waves, the Cohen Kappa coefficient was used. The correlation between the strength of agreement and the Kappa value was interpreted based on the ratings: < 0.00 (poor), 0.00 to 0.20 (negligible), 0.21 to 0.40 (weak), 0.41 to 0.60 (moderate), 0.61 to 0.80 (substantial), and 0.81 to 1.00 (almost perfect). The interpretation of the Interclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC) was based on the following classification: ICC < 0.4 (poor), ICC 0.4 to 0.75 (satisfactory), and ICC > 0.75 (excellent). Quantitative variables were described by means and standard deviations, and categorical variables by absolute and relative frequencies. A comparison of mean values between groups was performed using a student's t-test for independent groups in the case of symmetric variables, and a Mann-Whitney test for asymmetric variables. The results were computed using the IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) software, version 23.0, and the figures were generated in R version 4.0.3, accessed via RStudio version 1.3.1093 (Posit PBC, Boston, MA, USA), using the ggplot2 package. A level of 5% was adopted as the criterion for statistical significance (p ≤ 0.05).
Result
Sample Characterization
The current research involved 36 children (age group: 7–11 years; mean age: 8.89 years) divided into 2 groups, a CG and an EG, matched by sex, age, and handedness. [Table 1] characterizes the sample data.
There was a predominance of males (88.9%) and right-handed children (94.4%). In terms of stuttering severity, 11 children (61.1%) in the EG had mild stuttering, 6 (33.3%) moderate stuttering, and 1 (5.6%) had stuttering classified as severe.
Behavioral Assessment of Central Auditory Processing
[Table 2] describes the results of behavioral assessments of central auditory processing skill. There was a significant difference between groups for the RGDT, DDT, DPT, and PSI tests.
Abbreviations: DDT, dichotic digits test; DPT, duration pattern test; MLD, masking level difference; PSI, pediatric speech intelligibility; RGDT, random gap detection test.
Notes: Statistical significance (Fisher's exact test).
Electrophysiological Evaluation
[Table 3] presents the latency and amplitude results for waves P2 and P3. Statistically significant differences were observed regarding the latency of the P2 and P3 waves. For the P3 wave, statistically significant differences were observed in terms of both amplitude (p < 0.001) and latency (p < 0.001), in which there were longer latencies and smaller amplitudes in the group of stutterers.
Abbreviations: LE, left ear; RE, right ear; SD, standard deviation.
Notes: §, Student's t-test; *statistically significant.
Comparison between Behavioral and Electrophysiological Assessments
[Figs. 1] [2] [3] [4] [5] present the P3 electrophysiological findings for children who stutter, divided into those who passed a particular behavioral assessment and those who failed it (as set out in [Table 2]). On this basis, it is possible to see that there is a clear separation between the two categories for both the DTT and the DPT.










Agreement between Judges
There was excellent agreement between judges in the analysis of components of the P2 (Kappa = 0.88) and P3 (Kappa = 0.85) waves, as well as in the analysis of stuttering severity (Kappa = 0.82). Regarding the ICC, an ICC of 0.85 was obtained for P2 and 0.88 for P3, meaning almost perfect correlation.
Stuttering Severity
Because only one patient presented severe stuttering, this data was not taken into consideration when looking for correlations between electrophysiological findings and stuttering severity. However, in terms of those children with mild or moderate stuttering, a positive association was found between stuttering severity and increased latency of waves P2 (p < 0.001 in both ears) and P3 (RE: p = 0.015; LE: p = 0.048). This association was not found in the amplitude variable: for P2 the p-value for the RE was p = 0.216, and for the LE it was p = 0.180; similarly, for P3 the p-value for the RE was p = 0.098 and for the LE it was p = 0.301.
Scale of Auditory Behaviors
In the analysis of scores on the SAB questionnaire, all the children in the CG fell into the typical component category. However, in the EG, 10 (55.6%) children were typical, but 7 (38.9%) had low risk, and 1 (5.6%) had high risk of auditory processing alterations. Since only 1 child had high risk of auditory processing alterations, the correlation analysis between SAB scores and electrophysiological assessments was performed only with children having either a typical component or a low risk for auditory processing alterations. An association was found between SAB score and latency of waves P2 and P3, but not for P2 and P3 amplitude. The numbers for P2 latency were: RE, p = 0.025; LE, p = 0.043. For P3 latency, the corresponding figures were: RE, p = 0.007; LE, p = 0.003. The non-significant amplitude figures were for P2 (RE, p = 0.270; LE, p = 0.364) and for P3 (RE, p = 0.536; LE, p = 0.669).
Discussion
Stuttering, like other language disorders, largely affects males,[4] [7] and this was observed in the present study, in which 88.9% of the stuttering group was made up of boys.
In our results we found that both P2 and P3 had longer latencies in the experimental group—the stutterers—compared with the control group ([Table 3]). Similar findings have previously been found in adults[39] and in children,[40] indicating that stutterers may have a reduced cortical representation of acoustic stimuli and need more time to detect and discriminate sound inputs.
Regarding the amplitude of the P2 wave, no significant difference was observed between groups. One study performed on children who stutter found an increase in P2 amplitude only in the right ear[40] whereas another found a decrease in amplitude in adults.[39] No other studies appear in the reviewed literature which analyzed the amplitude of the P2 wave in children who stutter. In the sample studied, no change was observed in the amplitude of the P2 wave in children who stutter, indicating that children who stutter do not show a difference in the number of responsive neurons recruited to perform the task. It is suggested that further studies be performed on this topic.
Turning to P3, when results from the EG and CG were compared, differences were observed in terms of both the latency and amplitude of this wave, regardless of the ear stimulated ([Table 3]). Children who stuttered showed longer latencies, corroborating what is found in the literature.[19] [38] [39] [40] [41] These findings indicate that stutterers perceive acoustic parameters with less precision and with longer reaction times due to some defect in acoustic processing. Similarly, as described in the literature,[19] [38] [39] [40] [41] children from the EG had lower P3 amplitudes compared with those in the CG ([Table 3]). This may indicate that children who stutter require a greater neuron activation to differentiate and interpret auditory stimuli.
When non-linguistic stimuli are used in an oddball paradigm, P3 measurements reflect attention and memory skills.[24] [35] [40] Therefore, the findings of the present study suggest that the physiological processes devoted to attention and working memory are less robust in children who stutter. Previous studies of the performance of stutterers on behavioral tasks suggest that such individuals have difficulty focusing, regulating and sustaining attention, controlling attention in the presence of distracting background stimuli and conversations, and resisting or recovering from distractions.[50] [51] [52]
Regarding the behavioral assessment of central auditory processing skills (the RGDT, DDT, DPT, and PSI), the present study found lower performance of the EG when compared with the CG. This demonstrates that stutterers have difficulty in temporal resolution, figure-ground discrimination, binaural separation, duration discrimination, temporal ordering, and recognition of verbal sounds in monotic and dichotic listening. Other studies have also found deficits in behavioral assessments of central auditory processing in individuals who stutter.[18] [19] [20] [21] [22] [23] [53] It is believed that this inability to process information can contribute to the difficulty that people who stutter have in maintaining fluent speech.
In the comparison between the electrophysiological tests and behavioral findings in the EG, it was possible to see that there was a clear demarcation between P3 and the DPT and DDT ([Figs. 2] [3]), demonstrating that an increase in P3 latency is a strong indication of difficulties in acquiring or storing sequential information.[54] Individuals who stutter seem to need more time to elicit the P3 component, which impacts the speed of auditory sound processing, and this may explain the changes found here in DPT.
Since duration is an acoustic parameter that underlies the processing of uttered speech and heard voice, it is intimately connected to the rhythm and rate of speech.[18] Thus, children who stutter seem to have difficulty extracting the supra-segmental aspects of speech, and this may be a factor in causing disfluent speech.
Turning to the severity of stuttering, the majority of our EG was made up of children with mild and moderate levels of stuttering. A positive association was found between stuttering severity and increased latency of waves P2 and P3, suggesting that attention, discrimination, and memorization skills of acoustic stimuli may be related to speech fluency. A previous study performed with behavioral tests showed that the severity of stuttering is directly related to performance in tests of non-verbal auditory processing.[21] However, another study[55] failed to find the same correlation. In a literature search, we found no other studies that correlated the severity of stuttering with electrophysiological assessments.
Finally, regarding the results obtained on the SAB, we found that most children in the EG were identified as having typical auditory responses or having a low risk of alterations in their auditory processing. When the SAB scores were compared with the electrophysiological assessments, a positive correlation was observed between the SAB score and the latency of waves P2 and P3, Nevertheless, there is a need for more research in this area before one can say such a correlation is present in children at high risk of changes in auditory processing.
Conclusion
The findings of the present study provide evidence through electrophysiological and behavioral assessments that children who stutter may present changes in their abilities related to central auditory processing. The changes in latency and amplitude found in the analysis of P2 and P3 waves suggest that, even when not speaking, children who stutter exhibit electrophysiological differences in their AEPs. Knowledge about hearing difficulties in this population could contribute to the development of better therapeutic plans, including auditory training, with a view to improving auditory skills and improving speech fluency.
Conflict of Interests
The authors have no conflict of interests to declare.
Ethical Approval
The present research was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, under protocol number 41362. All participants and their guardians were informed about the procedures and signed a Free and Informed Consent Form.
Author's Contribution
Letícia Gregory was responsible for data collection, data analysis, and article writing. Milaine Dominici Sanfins, Rafael Fabiano Machado Rosa, and Piotr Henryk Skarzynski reviewed the article. Paulo Ricardo Gazzola Zen and Pricila Sleifer provided guidance and also reviewed the article.
-
References
- 1 Wingate ME. A standard definition of stuttering. J Speech Hear Disord 1964; 29: 484-489
- 2 Chang SE, Zhu DC. Neural network connectivity differences in children who stutter. Brain 2013; 136 (Pt 12): 3709-3726
- 3 Oliveira CM, Bohnen AJ. Diagnóstico Diferencial dos Distúrbios de Fluência. In: Lamônica AP, Britto DB. Tratado de linguagem: perspectivas contemporâneas. Ribeirão Preto: Book Toy; 2017: 5-82
- 4 Yairi E, Ambrose N. Epidemiology of stuttering: 21st century advances. J Fluency Disord 2013; 38 (02) 66-87
- 5 Reilly S, Onslow M, Packman A. et al. Natural history of stuttering to 4 years of age: a prospective community-based study. Pediatrics 2013; 132 (03) 460-467
- 6 Andrade CR. Gagueira desenvolvimental persistente. In: Andrade CR. Adolescentes e adultos com gagueira: fundamentos e aplicações clínicas. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2017: 5-10
- 7 Degiovani VM, Chiari BM, Schiefer AM. Disfluência: caracterização dos tipos e frequência de ocorrência em um grupo de escolares. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient 1999; 11 (01) 32-7
- 8 Ingham RJ, Fox PT, Ingham JC. et al. Functional-lesion investigation of developmental stuttering with positron emission tomography. J Speech Hear Res 1996; 39 (06) 1208-1227
- 9 Kroll RM, De Nil LF. Neural bases of stuttering and its treatment. In: SFA newsletter. Memphis: TN; 2000
- 10 Sandak R, Fiez JA. Stuttering: a view from neuroimaging. Lancet 2000; 356 (9228): 445-446
- 11 Van Borsel J, Sunaert R, Engelen S. Speech disruption under delayed auditory feedback in multilingual speakers. J Fluency Disord 2005; 30 (03) 201-217
- 12 Brown S, Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Laird AR, Fox PT. Stuttered and fluent speech production: an ALE meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 2005; 25 (01) 105-117
- 13 Chang SE, Zhu DC, Choo AL, Angstadt M. White matter neuroanatomical differences in young children who stutter. Brain 2015; 138 (Pt 3): 694-711
- 14 Weber-Fox C, Hampton Wray A, Arnold H. Early childhood stuttering and electrophysiological indices of language processing. J Fluency Disord 2013; 38 (02) 206-221
- 15 Desai J, Huo Y, Wang Z. et al. Reduced perfusion in Broca's area in developmental stuttering. Hum Brain Mapp 2017; 38 (04) 1865-1874
- 16 Pereira LD, Navas ALGP, Santos MTM. Processamento auditivo: uma abordagem de associação entre a audição e a linguagem. In: Santos MTM, Navas ALGP. Distúrbios de leitura e escrita: teoria e prática. Barueri: Manole; 2002: 75-95
- 17 Costa JB, Ritto AP, Juste FS, Andrade CRF. Comparison between the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter. CoDAS 2017; 29 (02) e20160136
- 18 Silva R, Oliveira CMC, Cardoso ACV. Aplicação dos testes de padrão temporal em crianças com gagueira desenvolvimental persistente. Rev CEFAC 2011; 13: 902-908
- 19 Prestes R, de Andrade AN, Santos RB, Marangoni AT, Schiefer AM, Gil D. Temporal processing and long-latency auditory evoked potential in stutterers. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol 2017; 83 (02) 142-146
- 20 Blood IM. Disruptions in auditory and temporal processing in adults who stutter. Percept Mot Skills 1996; 82 (01) 272-274
- 21 Andrade CRF, Schochat E. Comparação entre os achados neurolinguísticos e neuroaudiológicos das gagueiras. Pró-fono R Atual Cient 1999; 11: 27-30
- 22 Andrade NA, Gil D, Schiefer AM, Pereira LD. Processamento auditivo em gagos: análise do desempenho das orelhas direita e esquerda. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol 2008; 13 (01) 20-29
- 23 Andrade AN, Gil D, Schiefer AM, Pereira LD. Avaliação comportamental do processamento auditivo em indivíduos gagos. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient 2008; 20 (01) 43-8 . (2008b)
- 24 Hall JW. P300 response. In: Hall JW. New handbook of auditory evoked responses. Florida: Allyn & Bacon; 2006: 518-47
- 25 Kamita MK, Silva LAF, Matas CG. Potenciais evocados auditivos corticais no transtorno do espectro do autismo: revisão sistemática. CoDAS 2021; 33 (02) e20190207
- 26 Frizzo ACFA. Potenciais evocados auditivos de longa latência: conceitos e aplicações clínicas. In: Fernandes A. Tratado de Eletrofisiologia para a Audiologia. São Paulo: Book Toy; 2018: 139-50
- 27 McPherson DL. Late potentials of the auditory system. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group; 1996
- 28 Martin BA, Tremblay KL, Stapells DR. Principles and applications of cortical auditory evoked potentials. In: Burkard RF, Don M, Eggermont JJ. Auditory evoked potentials: basic principles and clinical application. Baltimore: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2007: 482-507
- 29 Almeqbel A. Speech-evoked cortical auditory responses in children with normal hearing. S Afr J Commun Disord 2013; 60: 38-43
- 30 Tremblay KL, Ross B, Inoue K, McClannahan K, Collet G. Is the auditory evoked P2 response a biomarker of learning?. Front Syst Neurosci 2014; 8: 28
- 31 García-Larrea L, Lukaszewicz AC, Mauguière F. Revisiting the oddball paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional effects. Neuropsychologia 1992; 30 (08) 723-741
- 32 Musiek FE, Froke R, Weihing J. The auditory P300 at or near threshold. J Am Acad Audiol 2005; 16 (09) 698-707
- 33 Sleifer P. Avaliação eletrofisiológica da audição em crianças. In: Cardoso MC. Fonoaudiologia na infância: avaliação e tratamento. Rio de Janeiro: Revinter; 2015: 171-94
- 34 Sanfins MD, Matas CG. Potencial Evocado Auditivo de Longa Latência (Peall): Potencial Cognitivo (P300). In: Menezes PL. et al, org. Manual de Eletrofisiologia e Eletroacústica Um Guia para Clínicos. Ribeirão Preto, São Paulo: Book Toy; 2022: 259
- 35 Jaeger A, Parente MAMP. Cognição e eletrofisiologia: uma revisão crítica das perspectivas nacionais. Psico-USF 2010; 15 (02) 171-180
- 36 Angrisani RMG, Matas CG, Neves IF, Sassi FC, Andrade CRF. Avaliação eletrofisiológica da audição em gagos, pré e pós terapia fonoaudiológica. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient 2009; 21: 95-100
- 37 Khedr E, El-Nasser WA, Abdel Haleem EK, Bakr MS, Trakhan MN. Evoked potentials and electroencephalography in stuttering. Folia Phoniatr Logop 2000; 52 (04) 178-186
- 38 Sassi FC, Matas CG, de Mendonça LI, de Andrade CR. Stuttering treatment control using P300 event-related potentials. J Fluency Disord 2011; 36 (02) 130-138
- 39 Hampton A, Weber-Fox C. Non-linguistic auditory processing in stuttering: evidence from behavior and event-related brain potentials. J Fluency Disord 2008; 33 (04) 253-273
- 40 Regaçone SF, Stenico MB, Gução ACB, Rocha ACM. Avaliação eletrofisiológica do sistema auditivo em indivíduos com gagueira desenvolvimental persistente. Rev CEFAC 2015; 17 (06) 1838-1847
- 41 Jerônimo GM, Scherer APR, Sleifer P. Potenciais evocados auditivos de longa latência em crianças com gagueira. Einstein (Sao Paulo) 2020; 18: 1-6
- 42 Maxfield ND, Olsen WL, Kleinman D, Frisch SA, Ferreira VS, Lister JJ. Attention demands of language production in adults who stutter. Clin Neurophysiol 2016; 127 (04) 1942-1960
- 43 Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2004
- 44 Andrade CRF. Protocolo para avaliação da fluência da fala. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient 2000; 12 (02) 131-4
- 45 Riley G. Stuttering Severity Instrument (SSI-4). Austin: Pro-Ed; 2009
- 46 ABA. Academia Brasileira de Audiologia. Fórum: diagnóstico audiológico – 2016. In: Anais do 31° Encontro Internacional de Audiologia. São Paulo: ABA; 2016
- 47 Souza J, Rocha VO, Berticelli AZ, Didoné DD, Sleifer P. Auditory latency response - P3 in children with and without learning complaints. Audiol Commun Res 2017; 22: e1690
- 48 Didoné DD, Garcia MV, Oppitz SJ. et al. Potencial evocado auditivo P300 em adultos: valores de referência. Einstein (Sao Paulo) 2016; 14 (02) 208-212
- 49 Nunes CL, Pereira LD, Carvalho GS. Scale of Auditory Behaviors e testes auditivos comportamentais para avaliação do processamento auditivo em crianças falantes do português europeu. CoDAS 2013; 25 (03) 209-215
- 50 Anderson JD, Wagovich SA. Aspects of attention in developmental stuttering. Presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA). Philadelphia; 2016
- 51 Eichorn N, Marton K, Pirutinsky S. Cognitive flexibility in preschool children with and without stuttering disorders. J Fluency Disord 2018; 57: 37-50
- 52 Ofoe LC, Anderson JD, Ntourou K. Short-Term Memory, Inhibition, and Attention in Developmental Stuttering: A Meta-Analysis. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2018; 61 (07) 1626-1648
- 53 Beinisis E. Resolução temporal em indivíduos com e sem gagueira. São Paulo: Universidade Federal de São Paulo; 2012
- 54 Pereira LD. Sistema auditivo e desenvolvimento das habilidades auditivas. In: Ferreira LP, Béfi-Lopes D, Limongi SCO. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. São Paulo: Roca; 2004: 547-52
- 55 Schiefer AM, Pereira LD, Barbosa LG. Considerações preliminares entre uma possível correlação entre gagueira e os aspectos linguísticos e auditivos. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient 1999; 11 (01) 27-31
Address for correspondence
Publication History
Received: 25 May 2024
Accepted: 02 January 2025
Article published online:
01 July 2025
© 2025. The Author(s). This is an open access article published by Thieme under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, permitting copying and reproduction so long as the original work is given appropriate credit (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)
Thieme Revinter Publicações Ltda.
Rua Rego Freitas, 175, loja 1, República, São Paulo, SP, CEP 01220-010, Brazil
Letícia Gregory, Milaine Dominici Sanfins, Piotr Henryk Skarzynski, Rafael Fabiano Machado Rosa, Paulo Ricardo Gazzola Zen, Pricila Sleifer. Auditory Processing in Stuttering Children: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Assessments. Int Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2025; 29: s00451804518.
DOI: 10.1055/s-0045-1804518
-
References
- 1 Wingate ME. A standard definition of stuttering. J Speech Hear Disord 1964; 29: 484-489
- 2 Chang SE, Zhu DC. Neural network connectivity differences in children who stutter. Brain 2013; 136 (Pt 12): 3709-3726
- 3 Oliveira CM, Bohnen AJ. Diagnóstico Diferencial dos Distúrbios de Fluência. In: Lamônica AP, Britto DB. Tratado de linguagem: perspectivas contemporâneas. Ribeirão Preto: Book Toy; 2017: 5-82
- 4 Yairi E, Ambrose N. Epidemiology of stuttering: 21st century advances. J Fluency Disord 2013; 38 (02) 66-87
- 5 Reilly S, Onslow M, Packman A. et al. Natural history of stuttering to 4 years of age: a prospective community-based study. Pediatrics 2013; 132 (03) 460-467
- 6 Andrade CR. Gagueira desenvolvimental persistente. In: Andrade CR. Adolescentes e adultos com gagueira: fundamentos e aplicações clínicas. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2017: 5-10
- 7 Degiovani VM, Chiari BM, Schiefer AM. Disfluência: caracterização dos tipos e frequência de ocorrência em um grupo de escolares. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient 1999; 11 (01) 32-7
- 8 Ingham RJ, Fox PT, Ingham JC. et al. Functional-lesion investigation of developmental stuttering with positron emission tomography. J Speech Hear Res 1996; 39 (06) 1208-1227
- 9 Kroll RM, De Nil LF. Neural bases of stuttering and its treatment. In: SFA newsletter. Memphis: TN; 2000
- 10 Sandak R, Fiez JA. Stuttering: a view from neuroimaging. Lancet 2000; 356 (9228): 445-446
- 11 Van Borsel J, Sunaert R, Engelen S. Speech disruption under delayed auditory feedback in multilingual speakers. J Fluency Disord 2005; 30 (03) 201-217
- 12 Brown S, Ingham RJ, Ingham JC, Laird AR, Fox PT. Stuttered and fluent speech production: an ALE meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 2005; 25 (01) 105-117
- 13 Chang SE, Zhu DC, Choo AL, Angstadt M. White matter neuroanatomical differences in young children who stutter. Brain 2015; 138 (Pt 3): 694-711
- 14 Weber-Fox C, Hampton Wray A, Arnold H. Early childhood stuttering and electrophysiological indices of language processing. J Fluency Disord 2013; 38 (02) 206-221
- 15 Desai J, Huo Y, Wang Z. et al. Reduced perfusion in Broca's area in developmental stuttering. Hum Brain Mapp 2017; 38 (04) 1865-1874
- 16 Pereira LD, Navas ALGP, Santos MTM. Processamento auditivo: uma abordagem de associação entre a audição e a linguagem. In: Santos MTM, Navas ALGP. Distúrbios de leitura e escrita: teoria e prática. Barueri: Manole; 2002: 75-95
- 17 Costa JB, Ritto AP, Juste FS, Andrade CRF. Comparison between the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter. CoDAS 2017; 29 (02) e20160136
- 18 Silva R, Oliveira CMC, Cardoso ACV. Aplicação dos testes de padrão temporal em crianças com gagueira desenvolvimental persistente. Rev CEFAC 2011; 13: 902-908
- 19 Prestes R, de Andrade AN, Santos RB, Marangoni AT, Schiefer AM, Gil D. Temporal processing and long-latency auditory evoked potential in stutterers. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol 2017; 83 (02) 142-146
- 20 Blood IM. Disruptions in auditory and temporal processing in adults who stutter. Percept Mot Skills 1996; 82 (01) 272-274
- 21 Andrade CRF, Schochat E. Comparação entre os achados neurolinguísticos e neuroaudiológicos das gagueiras. Pró-fono R Atual Cient 1999; 11: 27-30
- 22 Andrade NA, Gil D, Schiefer AM, Pereira LD. Processamento auditivo em gagos: análise do desempenho das orelhas direita e esquerda. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol 2008; 13 (01) 20-29
- 23 Andrade AN, Gil D, Schiefer AM, Pereira LD. Avaliação comportamental do processamento auditivo em indivíduos gagos. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient 2008; 20 (01) 43-8 . (2008b)
- 24 Hall JW. P300 response. In: Hall JW. New handbook of auditory evoked responses. Florida: Allyn & Bacon; 2006: 518-47
- 25 Kamita MK, Silva LAF, Matas CG. Potenciais evocados auditivos corticais no transtorno do espectro do autismo: revisão sistemática. CoDAS 2021; 33 (02) e20190207
- 26 Frizzo ACFA. Potenciais evocados auditivos de longa latência: conceitos e aplicações clínicas. In: Fernandes A. Tratado de Eletrofisiologia para a Audiologia. São Paulo: Book Toy; 2018: 139-50
- 27 McPherson DL. Late potentials of the auditory system. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group; 1996
- 28 Martin BA, Tremblay KL, Stapells DR. Principles and applications of cortical auditory evoked potentials. In: Burkard RF, Don M, Eggermont JJ. Auditory evoked potentials: basic principles and clinical application. Baltimore: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2007: 482-507
- 29 Almeqbel A. Speech-evoked cortical auditory responses in children with normal hearing. S Afr J Commun Disord 2013; 60: 38-43
- 30 Tremblay KL, Ross B, Inoue K, McClannahan K, Collet G. Is the auditory evoked P2 response a biomarker of learning?. Front Syst Neurosci 2014; 8: 28
- 31 García-Larrea L, Lukaszewicz AC, Mauguière F. Revisiting the oddball paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional effects. Neuropsychologia 1992; 30 (08) 723-741
- 32 Musiek FE, Froke R, Weihing J. The auditory P300 at or near threshold. J Am Acad Audiol 2005; 16 (09) 698-707
- 33 Sleifer P. Avaliação eletrofisiológica da audição em crianças. In: Cardoso MC. Fonoaudiologia na infância: avaliação e tratamento. Rio de Janeiro: Revinter; 2015: 171-94
- 34 Sanfins MD, Matas CG. Potencial Evocado Auditivo de Longa Latência (Peall): Potencial Cognitivo (P300). In: Menezes PL. et al, org. Manual de Eletrofisiologia e Eletroacústica Um Guia para Clínicos. Ribeirão Preto, São Paulo: Book Toy; 2022: 259
- 35 Jaeger A, Parente MAMP. Cognição e eletrofisiologia: uma revisão crítica das perspectivas nacionais. Psico-USF 2010; 15 (02) 171-180
- 36 Angrisani RMG, Matas CG, Neves IF, Sassi FC, Andrade CRF. Avaliação eletrofisiológica da audição em gagos, pré e pós terapia fonoaudiológica. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient 2009; 21: 95-100
- 37 Khedr E, El-Nasser WA, Abdel Haleem EK, Bakr MS, Trakhan MN. Evoked potentials and electroencephalography in stuttering. Folia Phoniatr Logop 2000; 52 (04) 178-186
- 38 Sassi FC, Matas CG, de Mendonça LI, de Andrade CR. Stuttering treatment control using P300 event-related potentials. J Fluency Disord 2011; 36 (02) 130-138
- 39 Hampton A, Weber-Fox C. Non-linguistic auditory processing in stuttering: evidence from behavior and event-related brain potentials. J Fluency Disord 2008; 33 (04) 253-273
- 40 Regaçone SF, Stenico MB, Gução ACB, Rocha ACM. Avaliação eletrofisiológica do sistema auditivo em indivíduos com gagueira desenvolvimental persistente. Rev CEFAC 2015; 17 (06) 1838-1847
- 41 Jerônimo GM, Scherer APR, Sleifer P. Potenciais evocados auditivos de longa latência em crianças com gagueira. Einstein (Sao Paulo) 2020; 18: 1-6
- 42 Maxfield ND, Olsen WL, Kleinman D, Frisch SA, Ferreira VS, Lister JJ. Attention demands of language production in adults who stutter. Clin Neurophysiol 2016; 127 (04) 1942-1960
- 43 Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2004
- 44 Andrade CRF. Protocolo para avaliação da fluência da fala. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient 2000; 12 (02) 131-4
- 45 Riley G. Stuttering Severity Instrument (SSI-4). Austin: Pro-Ed; 2009
- 46 ABA. Academia Brasileira de Audiologia. Fórum: diagnóstico audiológico – 2016. In: Anais do 31° Encontro Internacional de Audiologia. São Paulo: ABA; 2016
- 47 Souza J, Rocha VO, Berticelli AZ, Didoné DD, Sleifer P. Auditory latency response - P3 in children with and without learning complaints. Audiol Commun Res 2017; 22: e1690
- 48 Didoné DD, Garcia MV, Oppitz SJ. et al. Potencial evocado auditivo P300 em adultos: valores de referência. Einstein (Sao Paulo) 2016; 14 (02) 208-212
- 49 Nunes CL, Pereira LD, Carvalho GS. Scale of Auditory Behaviors e testes auditivos comportamentais para avaliação do processamento auditivo em crianças falantes do português europeu. CoDAS 2013; 25 (03) 209-215
- 50 Anderson JD, Wagovich SA. Aspects of attention in developmental stuttering. Presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA). Philadelphia; 2016
- 51 Eichorn N, Marton K, Pirutinsky S. Cognitive flexibility in preschool children with and without stuttering disorders. J Fluency Disord 2018; 57: 37-50
- 52 Ofoe LC, Anderson JD, Ntourou K. Short-Term Memory, Inhibition, and Attention in Developmental Stuttering: A Meta-Analysis. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2018; 61 (07) 1626-1648
- 53 Beinisis E. Resolução temporal em indivíduos com e sem gagueira. São Paulo: Universidade Federal de São Paulo; 2012
- 54 Pereira LD. Sistema auditivo e desenvolvimento das habilidades auditivas. In: Ferreira LP, Béfi-Lopes D, Limongi SCO. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. São Paulo: Roca; 2004: 547-52
- 55 Schiefer AM, Pereira LD, Barbosa LG. Considerações preliminares entre uma possível correlação entre gagueira e os aspectos linguísticos e auditivos. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient 1999; 11 (01) 27-31









