Gesundheitswesen 2012; 74 - A129
DOI: 10.1055/s-0032-1322115

Do people have a preference for increasing or decreasing pain? An experimental comparison of psychological and economic measures in health related decision making

J Trarbach 1
  • 1Otto-von-Guericke-Universität, Magdeburg

Scarce resources in the health care sector force decision makers to allocate treatments in a way that as much quality of life for as much people as possible is preserved. On an international level, the QALY concept is the most frequently used approach to measure quality of life.

One important factor for the measurement of quality of life is a good understanding of patients' preferences. Such preferences include, among others, the development of well-being in the course of a treatment. The QALY concept, based on Rational Choice Theory, assumes patients to be neutral in terms of this course, in case that the overall health state within a period is equal for two treatments. However, research, e.g. on the peak-end-rule (Kahneman et al. 1993), demonstrates the relevance of the distribution of health levels throughout the experience.

Results deviating from Rational Choice Theory mainly occur in studies using methods like questionnaires or rating scales, which exclude real consequences from decisions or answers. Research on the hypothetical bias emphasizes the importance of real consequences to gain reliable decisions of subjects.

In this study, we investigate whether the QALY assumption of neutrality towards the timing of health states is adequate. In particular we analyze if the described QALY assumption represents subjects' real preferences and if subjects are indifferent for the timing of painful events. We combine both, a questionnaire to evaluate bell-being and a standard economic measurement (WTP). We use monetary consequences and health-related consequences realized with the cold pressor test, a standard procedure to induce pain. Subjects in our experiments experience two painful sequences, with the same overall intensity but different temporal distributions of intensity.

We find that the questionnaire significantly reveals that subjects prefer one of the experiences. However, given real consequences subjects are indifferent on average. We conclude that although the rated difference between both experiences was clinically important the difference was not strong enough to be measureable with the other instrument using real consequences.

Our findings are relevant in terms of criticism on the QALY concept. Though some measurement methods indicate the relevance of the timing of well-being levels, the method we used including real consequences makes this preference disappear. Therefore the QALY assumption on neutrality towards timing was confirmed as correct and no adjustment is necessary to adequately represent subjects' preferences.

Literatur:

Amelang, Manfred & Zielinski, Werner 2002. Psychologische Diagnostik und Intervention. 3., korrigierte, aktualisierte und überarb. Aufl./unter Mitarb. von Thomas Fydrich ... Berlin [u.a.]: Springer.

Ariely, Dan 1998. Combining experiences over time: the effects of duration, intensity changes and on-line measurements on retrospective pain evaluations. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 11, 19–45.

Blumenschein, Karen, u.a. 2001. Hypothetical versus real willingness to pay in the health care sector: results from a field experiment. Journal of Health Economics 20, 441–457.

Breivik, Harald, u.a. 2006. Survey of chronic pain in Europe: Prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. European Journal of Health Economics 10(4), 287–333.

Camerer, Colin F. & Hogarth, Robin M. 1999. The Effects of Financial Incentives in Experiments: A Review and Capital-Labor-Production Framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 19(1–3), 7–42.

Choinière, Manon, u.a. 1990. Comparisons between patients'and nurses'assessment of pain and medication efficacy in severe burn injuries. Pain 40, 143–152.

Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. 1984. Discount Functions and the Measurement of Patients' Values: Women's Decisions During Childbirth. Medical Decision Making 4(1), 47–58.

Dagenais, S., Caro, J. & Haldeman, S. 2008. A systematic review of low back pain cost of illness studies in the United States and internationally. Spine Journal 8(1), 8–20.

Diener, E., Wirtz, D. & Oishi, S. 2001. End Effects of Rated Life Quality: The James Dean Effect. Psychological Science 12(2), 124–128.

Do, Amy M., Rupert, Alexander V. & Wolford, George 2008. Evaluations of pleasurable experiences: The peak-end rule. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 15(1), 96–98.

Dolan, Paul 2008. The measurement of health-related quality of life, in Culyer, Anthony J. & Newhouse, Joseph P. (Hg.): Handbook of health economics. Amsterdam [u.a.]: Elsevier, 1723–1762.

Dworkin, Robert H., u.a. 2005. Core outcome measures for chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. Pain 113, 9–19.

Eysenck, Michael W. & Keane, Mark T. 2010. Cognitive psychology: A student's handbook. 6. Aufl. Hove: Psychology Press.

Farrar, John T., u.a. 2001. Clinical importance of changes in chronic pain intensity measured on a 11-point numerical pain rating scale. Pain 94, 149–158.

Finn, Bridgid 2010. Ending on a high note: Adding a better end to effortful study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 36(6), 1548–1553.

Harrison, Glenn W. & Rutström, Elisabet 2008. Experimental Evidence on the Existence of Hypothetical Bias in Value Elicitation Methods. Handbook of Experimental Economics Results 1, 752–767.

Hertwig, Ralph & Ortmann, Andreas 2001. Experimental practices in economics: A methodological challenge for psychologists? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(3), 383–451.

Hines, Edgar A. & Brown, George E. 1936. The cold pressor test for measuring the reactibility of the blood pressure: data concerning 571 normal and hypertensive subjects. The American Heart Journal 11(1), 1–9.

Holt, Charles A. & Laury, Susan K. 2002. Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects. American Economic Review 92(5), 1644–1655.

Jensen, Mark P. 2003. The Validity and Reliability of Pain Measures in Adults with Cancer. The Journal of Pain 4(1), 2–21.

Kahneman, Daniel, Wakker, Peter P. & Sarin, Rakesh 1997. Back to Bentham? Explorations of Experienced Utility. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 112(2), 375–405.

Kahneman, Daniel, u.a. 1993. When more pain is preferred to less: Adding a better end. Psychological Science 4(6), 401–405.

Kühberger, Anton, Schulte-Mecklenbeck, Michael & Perner, Josef 2002. Framing Decisions: Hypothetical and real. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 89, 1162–1172.

Lee, Jennifer, Watson, David & Frey Law, Laura 2010. Lower-Order Pain-Related Constructs Are More Predictive of Cold Pressor Pain Ratings than Higher-Order Personality Traits. The Journal of Pain 11(7), 681–691.

Loewenstein, George F. & Prelec, Dražen 1993. Preferences for sequences of outcomes. Psychological Review 100(1), 91–108.

Loewenstein, George & Prelec, Drazen 1991. Negative Time Preference. The American Economic Review 81(2), 347–352.

Loewenstein, George & Sicherman, Nachum 1991. Do Workers Prefer Increasing Wage Profiles? Journal of Labor Economics 9(1), 67–95.

Lovallo, William 1975. The Cold Pressor Test and Autonomic Function: A Review and Integration. Psychophysiology 12(3), 268–282.

Miller, Norman & Campbell, Donald T. 1959. Recency and primacy in persuasion as a function of the timing of speeches and measurements. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 59(1), 1–9.

Müller, Holger, Kroll, Eike B. & Vogt, Bodo 2011. Do real payments really matter? A re-examination of the compromise effect in hypothetical and binding choice settings. Market Lett forthcoming.

Murdock, Bennet B. 1962. The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 64(5), 482–488.

Murphy, James J., u.a. 2005. A meta-analysis of hypothetical bias in stated preference valuation. Environmental and Resource Economics 30, 313–325.

Pesheva, Daniela, Kroll, Eike B. & Vogt, Bodo 2011. Gender differences in willingness to pay to avoid pain and their correlation with risk. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics 4(3)

181–191., Phillips, Ceri J. & Harper, Christie 2011. The economics associated with persistent pain. Current Opinion in Supportive and Palliative Care 5, 127–130.

Read, Daniel & Loewenstein, George 1999. Enduring pain for money: decisions based on the perception and memory of pain. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 12, 1–17.

Redelmeier, Donald A., Katz, J. & Kahneman, Daniel 2003. Memories of colonoscopy: a randomized trial. Pain 104(1–2), 187–194.

Redelmeier, Donald A. & Kahneman, Daniel 1996. Patient's memories of painful medical treatments: real-time and retrospective evaluations of two minimally invasive procedures. Pain 66, 3–8.

Samuelson, Paul A. 1937. A note on measurement of utility. The Revew of Economic Studies 4(2), 155–161.

Streff, Anouk, u.a. 2010. Differenzial physiological effects during tonic painful hand immersion tests using hot and ice water. European Journal of Pain 14(3), 266–272.

Thaler, Richard H. & Johnson, Eric J. 1990. Gambling with the House Money and Trying to Break Even: The Effects of Prior Outcomes on Risky Choice. Management Science 36(6), 643–660.

Varey, Carol & Kahneman, Daniel 1992. Experiences extended across time: Evaluation of moments and episodes. J. Behav. Decis. Making 5(3), 169–185.

Voelckner, Franziska 2006. An empirical comparison of methods for measuring consumers' willingness to pay. Market Lett 17(2), 137–149.

Weinstein, Milton C. & Stason, William B. 1977. Foundations of Cost-Effectiveness Analysis for Health and Medical Practices. New England Journal of Medicine 296, 716–721.

Wilcox, Nathaniel T. 1993. Lottery Choice: Incentives, Complexity and Decision Time. The Economic Journal 103, 1397–1417.